Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

Calmodulin-binding transcription factor shapes the male courtship song in Drosophila

  • Kosei Sato ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Kosei Sato, Md. Tanveer Ahsan

    Roles Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Neuro-Network Evolution Project, Advanced ICT Research Institute, National Institute of Information and Communications Technology, Kobe, Japan

  • Md. Tanveer Ahsan ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Kosei Sato, Md. Tanveer Ahsan

    Roles Funding acquisition, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Division of Neurogenetics, Tohoku University Graduate School of Life Sciences, Sendai, Japan

  • Manabu Ote,

    Roles Methodology, Supervision

    Affiliation Division of Neurogenetics, Tohoku University Graduate School of Life Sciences, Sendai, Japan

  • Masayuki Koganezawa,

    Roles Funding acquisition, Methodology, Resources

    Affiliation Division of Neurogenetics, Tohoku University Graduate School of Life Sciences, Sendai, Japan

  • Daisuke Yamamoto

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    daichan@nict.go.jp

    Affiliation Neuro-Network Evolution Project, Advanced ICT Research Institute, National Institute of Information and Communications Technology, Kobe, Japan

Abstract

Males of the Drosophila melanogaster mutant croaker (cro) generate a polycyclic pulse song dissimilar to the monocyclic songs typical of wild-type males during courtship. However, cro has not been molecularly mapped to any gene in the genome. We demonstrate that cro is a mutation in the gene encoding the Calmodulin-binding transcription factor (Camta) by genetic complementation tests with chromosomal deficiencies, molecular cloning of genomic fragments that flank the cro-mutagenic P-insertion, and phenotypic rescue of the cro mutant phenotype by Camta+-encoding cDNA as well as a BAC clone containing the gene for Camta. We further show that knockdown of the Camta-encoding gene phenocopies cro mutant songs when targeted to a subset of fruitless-positive neurons that include the mcALa and AL1 clusters in the brain. cro-GAL4 and an anti-Camta antibody labeled a large number of brain neurons including mcALa. We conclude that the Camta-encoding gene represents the cro locus, which has been implicated in a species-specific difference in courtship songs between D. sechellia and simulans.

Author summary

Selecting a suitable mate is a prerequisite for successful breeding in organisms. Indeed, the animals instinctively distinguish a conspecific partner from individuals of other species, yet the mechanism underlying such species-recognition remains largely unknown. In choosing a conspecific male as a mate, fruit fly females rely on a male-derived auditory signal, love song, which is generated by a series of unilateral wing vibration by the male. We study how the males produce love song that is unique to the species. We particularly focus on croaker (cro) mutants, whose males generate distorted love song. Our molecular analysis reveals that the cro mutation inhibits expression of the gene encoding a protein called Calmodulin-binding transcription factor (Camta) and that an introduction of the Camta-encoding DNA into the genome of cro mutants allows the mutant male to sing a normal song. Therefore, the Camta protein is an essential component for love song generation by males. We further show that knockdown of Camta only in tens of specific neurons in the brain is sufficient for inducing the cro mutant phenotype. This study paves the way for unraveling the mechanistic basis for female-male communications in conspecific mating.

Introduction

Premating isolation between subpopulations within a species is a hallmark of incipient speciation [1]. A prerequisite to successful premating isolation is the development of a directed bias in mate choice among the members of the species. The pair of species recently separated may still be able to produce offspring between them, yet the offspring is typically infertile, representing postmating isolation [2]. Drosophila fruit flies offer a tractable system for studying the mechanistic basis for premating isolation due to diversified mate-choice within a population, because this genus includes the genetic model species D. melanogaster, which is highly amenable to molecular and cellular analyses for complex traits, and also because D. melanogaster has a few sibling species, with which it can produce inter-species hybrids under certain conditions, despite a strong mate choice for a conspecific partner [24].

Mate choice in D. melanogaster relies on multiple sensory cues [5, 6]. For example, female-derived pheromones, particularly 7,11-heptacosadiene, are important for males to discriminate conspecific females from other species [7], whereas willingness of females to mate is markedly modulated by male-derived auditory stimuli known as courtship songs [8]. Choosiness of females has been suggested to operate as one of the major drives for the assortative mating underlying premating isolation, and hence, diversification in male song patterns may result from sexual selection for favorable males [9]. These considerations point to the importance of elucidating the genetic architecture of courtship song patterning mechanisms for the understanding of behavioral evolution.

The courtship song of D. melanogaster males is generated by wing vibration [10]. The male fly laterally extends one of two wings and rapidly moves it along the anterior-posterior direction for a few seconds, followed by a series of similar vibrations made by the other wing. He alternates wings to vibrate several times in a courtship epoch directed to a target female. Phonetic recordings of courtship songs reveal two distinct song types, i.e., pulse song and sine song [11]. The pulse song is a burst of spikes with an interpulse-interval (IPI) of ~35 ms in D. melanogaster [12]. A recent analysis has captured two modes of pulse song, slow pulse and fast pulse, whose carrier frequencies are 200–250 Hz and 250–400 Hz, respectively [13]. The sine song represents sustained oscillations similar to sine waves with carrier frequencies of 120–180 Hz [11]. A classic study showed that wingless silent males succeed in copulation only at a low rate, although this poor copulation-success rate is mitigated by exposing the female to an artificial sine song [14]. Pre-exposure of females to an artificial pulse song having correct IPIs facilitates female acceptance of courting males to mate [8]. Thus, both song quality and quantity impact mating success in D. melanogaster.

The circuit for male singing has been explored in some detail. The P1 interneurons involved in courtship decision-making [15] drive, upon integrating multiple sensory inputs, P2b [16] and pIP10 [17] descending interneurons to activate the thoracic song motor pattern generator (MPG), which is composed of vPR6, vMS11, dPR1 and other interneuron groups intrinsic to the thoracic ganglia [17, 18]. When activated artificially, P1 neurons preferentially induce sine songs, whereas pIP10 neurons preferentially induce fast pulse songs [13]. In contrast to these higher brain neurons with song-triggering roles, thoracic neurons such as vPR6 and vMS11 appear to be involved in patterning of pulse songs: vPR6 artificial activation shortens IPIs; vMS11 induces unilateral wing extension without wing vibration when activated, whereas it converts monocyclic songs to polycyclic ones when inactivated [17]. The ps1 neurons are yet another component of the song motor center [19], and have been implicated in the switching between fast and slow pulse song modes [13]. Song motor outputs thus patterned are delivered to direct and indirect muscles in the thoracic segments to orchestrate wing movements for the production of a specific song type [20]. Although major neural elements for song generation have been determined as above, the detailed circuit mechanisms for the song-controlling network remain obscure.

In elucidating the neural organization and action principles of the song circuit, it is indispensable to understand the molecular constituents that play key roles in the establishment and operation of the circuit. Molecular changes in building blocks of the circuit must underlie the divergence in song characteristics among different species. Genetic screens for mutants with song pattern defects would seem to be a promising approach in unbiased searches for genes involved in song production. However, only a few mutants with abnormal courtship song structures have been isolated. cacophony (cac), a pioneering member of such mutants [12], is characterized by polycyclic pulse song and mapped to the voltage gated calcium channel gene DMCA1A [21]. Another mutation reported to disturb the song pattern is dissonance (diss), and males with this mutation also produce polycyclic pulse songs [22]. The non on or off transient A (nonA) gene responsible for the diss mutation encodes an RNA binding protein [23], which is likely required for synaptic transmission from photoreceptors as inferred from the aberrant electroretinogram (ERG) in diss mutants. We have isolated a P-element insertion mutant with reduced mating success, which is ascribable to defects in males but not females [24]: males of this mutant sing abnormally and thus we named it croaker (cro) [24]. In cro mutants, pulse song is often polycyclic, IPIs are prolonged, and the sine song amplitude within a bout is more variable [24]. However, the molecular identification of the phenotypically defined cro locus has been hampered by the presence of a long array of repetitive DNA sequences around the mutagenic P-element insertion. One aim of this study is to molecularly identify the gene responsible for the cro mutation. Upon obtaining the cro sequence, we can endeavor to unravel where, when and how the functional cro product shapes male courtship songs, using molecular probes for cro. This achievement of unraveling the molecular underpinning of cro effects on courtship songs may also shed light on the evolutionary basis for the diversification in song patterns, because cro is the locus that has been implicated in the species-difference of pulse song IPIs between D. simulans and D. sechellia by a Quantitative Trait Loci (QTL) analysis [25].

Results

Because the cro mutant stock has been maintained for nearly 30 years without use, we first outcrossed it to a control w1118 strain for five generations to standardize the genetic background and examined whether the phenotype originally found in the cro stock can be detected in the newly outcrossed stock. We focus on the pulse song phenotype as it is highly amenable to quantitative evaluation. Indeed, the IPI prolongation reported in the original publication [24] was reproduced in this study (Fig 1A–1E). We also confirmed that the cro mutant males often generate polycyclic pulse song [24], which is practically absent in control male song (Fig 1A, 1F and 1G). We therefore count the incidence of polycyclic pulses as a read out of the cro mutant phenotype. The cro mutagenic P-insertion has been mapped at the cytological position 45DE by polytene in situ hybridization with a genomic fragment near the P-element insertion point [24]. Based on this information, we carried out genetic complement tests of the cro mutation with several deficiency chromosomes (Dfs) carrying break points in this cytological region. Among the 5 Dfs examined, Dfw73-2 prevented polycyclic pulses from occurring, whereas Dfs Np5, w45-30n, ED1791 and BSC408 did not when placed in trans to the cro chromosome (Fig 1G and 1H), positioning the cro locus between the cytological divisions 45D8 and 45E3 (Fig 1H).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Genetic complementation tests identify the Camta-encoding region to be responsible for the cro mutant song phenotype.

(A) Typical examples of pulse songs from males of the control w1118 (upper trace) and cro mutant (lower trace). (B-E) The interpulse interval (IPI) is susceptible to the cro mutation. (B-D) Distribution of IPIs in the control w1118 (B) and cro homozygote without (C) or with (D) the genomic rescue fragment BAC-CH321-22B08. The Gaussian fit to the IPI distribution is drawn on each histogram, and in (E) three Gaussian curves are superimposed on the same panel to facilitate comparisons. The statistical differences were evaluated by one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s multiple comparisons; *: P<0.05. (F) Criteria used to classify the recorded pulses as monocyclic or polycyclic. The peak height of the song pulse (the peak) is measured from the baseline and the 50% of this value is chosen as the slice level. When any deflections other than the primary peak exceed the slice level, the pulse is judged to be polycyclic. When the peak is the only deflection that exceeds the slice level, the pulse is judged to be monocyclic. (G) The incidence of polycyclic pulses compared among the genotypes indicated. The number of song pulses used to calculate the proportion of polycyclic pulses is indicated in parentheses above the bars. The proportion of polycyclic pulses is shown. The statistical significance between the control and a test group is evaluated by the Fisher’s exact probability test. ***: P<0.001, NS: not significant. (H) A diagrammatic representation of the genomic regions (cytology is at the top) that are deleted in deficiency chromosomes. The region covered by BAC-CH312-22B08 and the genes and their transcriptional directions are indicated by pointed bars in an expanded scale.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1008309.g001

In this cytological interval, three genes with known functions have been annotated, i.e., bruchpilot (brp), Wnt2 and Camta, together with additional less characterized genes as indicated in Fig 1H. To narrow our search for the genomic region responsible for the cro phenotype, we attempted to rescue the cro song defects by introducing, into the cro mutant line, a Bac clone containing a complete locus for Camta (BAC-CH321-22B08) but not functional Wnt2 and brp (Fig 1H). Because BAC-CH321-22B08 was able to rescue the song defects in cro mutants (Fig 1D–1G), Camta, but not Wnt2 and brp, remained a candidate for the gene responsible for the song phenotype.

As an independent approach to the identification of the cro locus, we attempted to identify molecularly the P-insertion point in cro mutants. We took two different strategies to recover a sequence adjacent to the P-insertion site, i.e., plasmid rescue in E. coli and cloning by inverse PCR. These two approaches yielded an identical sequence that flanks the mutagenic P-element in cro mutants, which matched an intronic sequence in the locus encoding Camta (Fig 2A and 2B).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Transposon insertions into the Camta-encoding gene and their ability to induce the song phenotype when heterozygous over cro.

(A) The unique sequence at the mutagenic P-element insertion in the cro mutant genome. The sequence cloned by plasmid rescue and inverse PCR is underlined. (B) The schematic drawings of the exon-intron organization for Camta splice forms and the transposon insertion points. The BmΔw insertion responsible for the original cro mutation is boxed. The positions of sequences used as primers for qPCR are indicated at the bottom and also shown in an inset between panels B and D in an expanded scale. (C) The incidence of polycyclic pulses compared among the genotypes indicated. The number of song pulses used to calculate the proportion of polycyclic pulses is indicated in parentheses above the bars. The proportion of polycyclic pulses is shown. The statistical significance between the control and a test group is evaluated by the Fisher’s exact probability test. ***: P<0.001, NS: not significant. (D) Quantitative comparisons of Camta mRNA levels among the genotypes indicated. CamtaMI01580, which do not complement cro, as well as Df(2R)BSC408 reduce Camta transcripts when placed in trans to cro. The statistical significance between the control and a test group is evaluated by the Mann-Whitney U test. ***: P<0.001, **: P<0.01, NS: not significant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1008309.g002

We note that cac, one of a few genes that induce polycyclic pulse song when mutated, encodes a voltage-dependent Ca2+ channel [21], implying that Ca2+ is important for the pulse song shaping, to which Camta could also contribute as a Ca2+-responsive transcriptional regulator. Several fly lines carrying a transposon insertion in the locus encoding Camta are publicly available (Fig 2B). We tested these five potential alleles of the cro locus for their ability to induce polycyclic pulse song when placed in trans to cro: Mi{MIC}CamtaMI01580 and Mi{MIC}CamtaMI04570 produced polycyclic pulse song, whereas Mi{MIC}CamtaMI01132A, PBac(WH)Camtaf05489 and Mi{ET1}CamtaMB05101 did not (Fig 2C). We note that the three effective insertions, cro, Mi{MIC}CamtaMI01580 and Mi{MIC}CamtaMI04570, occurred at the middle of the locus, i.e., the region from exon 4 to exon 6, in contrast to the three ineffective transposon insertions, which were located more proximally (Mi{MIC}CamtaMI01132A) or distally (PBac(WH)Camtaf05489 and Mi{ET1}CamtaMB05101) in the locus (Fig 2B). We inferred that the effective insertions reduce functional transcripts for Camta, thereby impairing the production of normal pulse song. Indeed, qPCR with primers specific for exon 6 and exon 7 sequences revealed a marked reduction in transcript levels for Camta in cro/Mi{MIC}CamtaMI04570 as well as cro homozygotes and cro/Df(2R)BSC408 (Fig 2D). None of these heteroallelic mutants caught our attention as displaying a phenotype qualitatively different from the phenotypes of the original cro mutants. Taking all these results together, we consider that the Camta-encoding transcription unit likely represents the cro locus, loss of which results in the production of polycyclic pulse song.

To further clarify the relationship between the Camta-encoding locus and cro, we generated a GAL4-expressing allele by replacing the MIMIC cassette with the GAL4-encoding sequence, i.e., T2A-GAL4 [26], at the MI04570 insertion in the cro locus (S1 Fig see also Fig 2B). The cro-GAL4 allele thus obtained was lethal, yielding no adult fly. We therefore generated cro/cro-GAL4 heteroallelic males, which turned out to produce polycyclic song pulses (Fig 3A and 3B). The polycyclic pulses often intermingled with monocyclic pulses within a song bout; a single bout composed of exclusively monocyclic pulses or exclusively polycyclic pulses was rare (Fig 3A). The polycyclic pulses were typically of two (~65%) or three cycles (~35%), with a few exceptional pulses of more than four cycles (less than 1%). We also introduced UAS-Camta+ into this heteroallelic mutant, and found that the incidence of polycyclic pulses was markedly reduced (Fig 3B). This result convincingly demonstrates that deficits in the Camta-encoding gene are responsible for the song defects in cro mutants.

thumbnail
Fig 3. cro/cro-GAL4 mutant song defects are rescued by Camta+ overexpression.

(A) Examples of courtship song records from a cro/cro-GAL4 mutant male. Three representative traces obtained from a single fly are shown. Pulses classified as polycyclic are marked with asterisks. (B) cro affects the incidence of polycyclic pulses. Males of w1118 and cro-GAL4 rarely generate polycyclic pulses, whereas more than 30% of pulses in cro/cro-GAL4 males are polycyclic. Overexpression of Camta+ rescues the mutant song phenotype. The number of pulses examined is shown in parentheses. The statistical significance of differences was evaluated by one-way ANOVA followed by Fisher’s exact probability tests; **: P<0.01. (C-F) Distribution of IPIs in the control cro-GAL4/+ (C) and cro/cro-GAL4 without (D) or with (E) the UAS-transgene that overexpresses Camta+. The Gaussian fit to the IPI distribution is drawn on each histogram, and in (F) three Gaussian curves and a control w1118 curve reproduced from Fig 1E are superimposed on the same panel to facilitate comparisons. Pulses were detected automatically as events that exceeded the slice level. In this series of experiments, the slice threshold was set at a more stringent level of 60% to avoid erroneous counts of non-song deflections due to lower signal-to-noise ratio. Even under such stringent conditions, the estimated incidence of polycyclic pulses was similar to that obtained at a slice level of 50%.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1008309.g003

Because cro/cro-GAL4 provides a useful platform for documenting and potentially rescuing mutant phenotypes, we focused on this genotype for our further analyses of song characteristics. The interpulse interval (IPI) was significantly prolonged in the cro/cro-GAL4 song, and Camta+ overexpression restored the normal IPI in the song of this mutant (Fig 3C–3F). We then determined the carrier frequency for song pulses by fast Fourier transformation. Song pulses recorded from w1118 control males exhibited a conspicuous peak of spectrum density at 234.3Hz, in keeping with previous reports (Fig 4A and 4D). The power spectrum for cro/cro-GAL4 pulses had its highest peak at 312.5 Hz, accompanied by additional, smaller peaks at even higher frequencies (Fig 4A). To evaluate the contribution of monocyclic and polycyclic pulses to the altered frequency characteristics in cro/cro-GAL4, we separately obtained a power spectrum for each pulse type. Monocyclic pulses resulted in a power spectrum with a peak at 312.5 Hz, followed by a monotonic decline in the power density at higher frequencies (Fig 4B). In contrast, polycyclic pulses yielded a power spectrum with the highest peak at 351.5 Hz, accompanied by multiple humps at the higher frequency range (Fig 4C). These observations indicate that the carrier frequency of mutant pulse songs is shifted to higher frequencies even in monocyclic pulses, though the magnitude of shift is smaller than in polycyclic pulses, and that multiple peaks at the higher frequency range are derived from polycyclic pulses. It is of interest to note that a power spectrum with similar multiple peaks has been reported for diss mutants, which also generates polycyclic pulse song [27]. All these defects in the carrier frequency of song pulses were fully rescued by Camta+ overexpression (Fig 4A and 4D). We conclude that the Camta-encoding gene is responsible for the cro song phenotypes.

thumbnail
Fig 4. cro affects carrier frequency characteristics of song pulses.

(A-C) Power spectra of pulse songs recorded from male flies of w1118 (A: black) and cro/cro-GAL4 without (A-C: red) or with (A: blue) UAS-Camta+. The drawn line represents a power spectrum of traces with no song recorded from cro/cro-GAL4, which is indistinguishable from similar spectra of traces containing no song (i.e., noise only) obtained from other genotypes. cro/cro-GAL4 monocyclic (B) and polycyclic (C) pulses separately subjected to fast Fourier transformation yield distinctly different power spectra. Spectra illustrated in (B) and (C) but not (A) have been noise-subtracted. Perpendicular broken lines indicate the maximal peak frequency. (D) The maximal peak frequency estimated in different flies of the indicated genotypes is compared. The means ± SEM are indicated by horizontal and vertical bars superimposed on the data points. The statistical significance of differences was evaluated by Kruskal-Wallis analysis followed by Steel-Dwass nonparametric multiple comparisons; **: P<0.01.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1008309.g004

As expected, cro-GAL4-driven Camta knockdown reduced Camta mRNA expression and yielded male flies that generated polycyclic pulse song during courtship (Fig 5A and 5C). To determine the tissue in which the Camta-encoding cro gene functions for the shaping of pulse song, we carried out knockdown of the Camta-encoding gene as targeted to neurons, a cell type where cro likely functions in male flies to generate normal pulse song. Two neural drivers, elavGAL4 and nSybGAL4, both induced polycyclic song, whereas the glial driver repoGAL4 did not (Fig 5B), supporting the hypothesis that the gene for Camta is responsible for the cro song defects and is required in neurons for normal song generation. In accord with this hypothesis, cro-GAL4 was widely expressed in the nervous system at the larval, pupal and adult stages (Fig 5D–5H; S2A–S2E Fig). A newly generated antibody against Camta (Fig 6A) labeled a large number of cells in the brain, the staining intensity of which was reduced in cro/cro-GAL4 mutants (Fig 6B vs. 6C). The majority of brain cells positive for the anti-Camta antibody expressed cro-GAL4, indicating that cro-GAL4 faithfully recapitulates endogenous Camta expression (Fig 6D–6F).

thumbnail
Fig 5. cro knockdown in cro-GAL4-positive neurons induces polycyclic pulses.

(A and B) Knockdown of Camta via cro-GAL4 (A), elavGAL4 and nSybGAL4 but not repo-GAL4 (B) induces polycyclic song. The proportion of polycyclic pulses was compared among the genotypes indicated. The tested flies carry transgenes marked with + at the bottom. (B) fruGAL4 but not dsxGAL4 is effective in inducing the song phenotype when used as a GAL4 driver. The proportion of polycyclic pulses (%) is shown in the bar chart and the number of pulses analyzed is indicated in parentheses. Statistical significance was evaluated by the Fisher’s exact probability test. ***: P< 0.001; NS: not significant. (C) qPCR reveals a reduction of Camta transcripts by RNAi-mediated knockdown as driven by Act5C-GAL4 or cro-GAL4. The flies examined carried the transgenes indicated by the plus mark below the box plot graph. The number of replicates is shown in parentheses. The box plot shows the median and 10th, 25th, 75th, and 90th percentiles. The statistical significance of differences was evaluated by Kruskal-Wallis analysis followed by Steel’s nonparametric multiple comparisons: *: P<0.05. (D-H) cro-GAL4 expression is widespread in the central nervous system at the third instar larva (D), wandering stage larva (E), pupa (F), and adult stages (brain: G; VNC: H). Scale bars: 50 μm (D-H).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1008309.g005

thumbnail
Fig 6. Expression of cro-GAL4 recapitulates endogenous Camta expression.

(A) The antibody recognizes a unique sequence in Camta, whose domain structure is schematically illustrated. (B and C) The entire brain stained positive for Camta in wild-type (B) and cro/cro-GAL4 (C) males. (D-F) High magnitude images of the periesophageal region highlight cell bodies doubly positive for GFP (D and F: cro-GAL4, green) and Camta (E and F, magenta). Scale bars: 50 μm (B; also applicable to C) or 10 μm (D; also applicable to E and F).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1008309.g006

The fact that a neuronally targeted knockdown of the gene encoding Camta phenocopied cro mutants suggests an essential role of Camta in neurons for song patterning. To determine which subset of CNS cells requires functional cro in shaping pulse song, we performed cro knockdown in fruGAL4- or dsxGAL4-positive cells, because the core portion of the male courtship circuit is composed of fru- and/or dsx-expressing neurons [18, 28]. Notably, fruGAL4-driven Camta RNAi expression, but not dsxGAL4-driven Camta RNAi expression, induced polycyclic pulse song, indicating that fruGAL4-positive neurons contribute, at least in part, to cro-dependent shaping of pulse song patterns (Fig 7A). It has been postulated that courtship decisions are made in the brain [15], whereas the motor pattern generator for courtship behavior resides in the ventral nerve cord (VNC) [17]. To clarify which of these centers (brain vs. VNC) requires cro for the production of monocyclic pulse song, we restricted Camta RNAi expression to fruGAL4-positive neurons in the brain by the use of OtdFLP [29], which releases GAL4 from GAL80-mediated repression by flipping the GAL80 sequence (>GAL80>) out from tub>GAL80> only in the head. OtdFLP effectively restricted fruGAL4-driven UAS-mCD8::GFP expression in the brain (S3 Fig). We found a significant increase in the incidence of polycyclic pulse song in male flies in which cro knockdown was restricted to fruGAL4-positive neurons in the brain (Fig 7B; S2F–S2K Fig), supporting the notion that cro-positive fru-expressing brain neurons play an important role for pulse song shaping. However, polycyclic pulses were less frequent when cro knockdown was restricted to the brain fru-expressing neurons than when cro knockdown was achieved in wider domains of tissues as driven by, for example, cro-GAL4, elavGAL4 or nSybGAL4(Fig 5B cf. Fig 7B). fru-positive neurons in the VNC, fru-negative cells in the nervous system and even non-neuronal cells might also be involved, to some extent, in the song defects.

thumbnail
Fig 7. cro functions in the fruGAL4-positive but not dsxGAL4-positive population of neurons for normal song production.

(A) fruGAL4 but not dsxGAL4 is an effective GAL4 driver for cro knockdown in inducing the song phenotype. (B) When OtdFLP was used to restrict the effects of fruGAL4 on neurons in the brain, polycyclic pulse generation was not inhibited, suggesting that brain fru-expressing neurons are involved in the observed song phenotype. (C) The combination of p52a-GAL4 and fruFLP allows mcALa-specific expression of Camta RNAi, resulting in the polycyclic pulse generation. P1-specific NP2631-GAL4 and pIP10-specific VT40556-GAL4 are without effect on the pulse song shape when used as drivers for Camta RNAi. (D) Brain neurons visualized by UAS>stop>mCD8::GFP as driven by p52a-GAL4 in the presence of fruFLP. mcALa neurons and a few additional fru-positive clusters including AL1 are stained. (E-H) cro-GAL4 expression in the nervous system includes fru-positive mcALa and AL1 neurons. cro-CAL4 and fruFLP intersection visualized mcALa and AL1 neurons. mcALa (arrows) and AL1 (arrowheads) are shown in the entire brain (E) or in enlarged views (F and G). AL1 in the right hemisphere (F) and bilateral mcALa (G) are delineated with white broken lines. The intersection also stained a large number of fru-GAL4-positive neurons in the VNC (H). The antibodies used for staining are indicated above each panel. Scale bars: 50 μm (D, E and H) or 10 μm (F and G).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1008309.g007

Taking into account the observation that the head-restricted cro knockdown in fruGAL4-expressing neurons induced polycyclic pulse song, we next tested whether cro knockdown in a few fru-expressing brain neurons known to have a pivotal role in courtship song generation can induce polycyclic pulse song. We first examined pIP10 neurons which represent laterally paired descending interneurons with a command fiber-like function to initiate male courtship upon artificial activation. These interneurons have been reported to trigger pulse song generation when forcibly activated [13]. Notably, cro knockdown specifically targeted to pIP10 neurons by VT40556-GAL4 did not induce polycyclic pulse song during male courtship toward a female (Fig 7C). Next, we examined the effect of cro knockdown in mcALa neurons, which have been implicated in the ordering of courtship elementary actions [30]. Interestingly, the mcALa-specific intersectional combination p52a-GAL4 and fruFLP led to the generation of polycyclic pulse song (Fig 7C and 7D), suggesting a new role for mcALa, i.e., shaping pulse song. We note, however, that this intersectional combination also induced Camta RNAi expression in a few additional fru-positive cells, including AL1. We cannot exclude the possibility that these cells, rather than mcALa, are responsible for the polycyclic pulse song generation. In contrast, P1-targetted cro knockdown by the intersection of NP2631-GAL4 and fruFLP did not induce polycyclic pulse song during male courtship (Fig 7C). Importantly, we detected cro-GAL4 expression in mcALa and AL1 by the intersection of cro-GAL4 and fruFLP (Fig 7E–7H). The anti-Camta antibody strongly labeled cell bodies around the suboesophageal ganglion (SOG), which appeared to include those of mcALa (Fig 6D–6F). We were unable to distinguish AL1 somata from surrounding cell bodies labeled by the anti-Camta antibody. We conclude that cro is required in some fru-expressing neurons, including mcALa, and that the polycyclic nature of pulse song is a manifestation of malfunction of these neurons that constitute the core brain circuit for male courtship behavior.

Discussion

Camta represents a conserved family of proteins known to exist in a wide range of phyla from plants to mammals [31, 32]. The Camta proteins are characterized by the DNA-binding CG-1 domain at the N-terminus and the IQ domain for interacting with Calmodulin at the C-terminus [33, 34]. Camta has been shown to bind to a DNA motif known as the CGCG-box and transcriptionally activates a nearby gene [33, 34]. In Drosophila, Camta has been shown to promote expression of dFbxl4, which encodes a transcription factor required for rapid deactivation of rhodopsin [35, 36].

In this study, we presented evidence that the polycyclic distortion of pulse song in cro mutant males originates from defects in the fru-labeled circuit underlying male courtship behavior. Of note, loss of cro from mcALa neurons was sufficient to induce a discernible change in pulse song. mcALa neurons originate from the SOG and send midline ascending axons to the dorsal protocerebrum, contributing part of the so-called arch [37]. An intriguing possibility is that synaptic connections formed by these neurons with other cells within the courtship circuit are disturbed by loss of cro. Another interesting possibility is that mcALa neurons need to acquire some physiological properties essential for the song shaping function.

Whereas mcALa neurons are confined in the brain-SOG complex (Fig 7D), song motor outputs are likely produced in the MPG localized in the thoracic ganglia [17]. It is envisaged that neurons composing the MPG require, in order to shape pulse song, mcALa-derived tuning signals, which are probably relayed as yet unidentified descending neurons to the MPG from the brain-SOG complex [38, 39]. Ding et al. [40] recently showed that artificial activation of pIP10 descending neurons can produce a high frequency clack song, similar to that generated by D. yakuba males, in D. melanogaster, which usually do not produce this type of song. They postulated that fine adjustments of the existing descending system, e.g., strengthening synaptic connections with an otherwise silent MPG, could result in a new behavioral trait (e.g., song) in a derived species through evolution [40]. By analogy, one might envisage that cro is required in order to adjust the strength of connectivity between mcALa neurons and a descending system that drives a pulse song MPG. In light of the suggested mcALa function, i.e., aligning courtship elements in sequence, this neuronal group is anticipated to have access to multiple MPGs, making it an ideal site where quantitative changes in a courtship action are produced. In regard to the contracted courtship behavior reported to occur upon fru knockdown in mcAL (including mcALa) neurons [30], mcALa-targeted Camta knockdown or cro mutations did not impair the sequential execution of courtship behavior. It might be that, in shaping pulse song, cro functions independently of fru gene products, which are nonetheless required for ordering courtship actions in correct sequence. The precise characterization of deficits in neural structure and function caused by cro loss is an urgent issue to be addressed.

The song structure and organization are species-specific and such divergence in courtship songs is indeed key for reproductive isolation among species [41, 42]. One of the most striking cases of song-dependent sexual isolation is found between D. montana and D. lummei, two closely related species that are sympatric in some habitats, which do not mate with each other in nature [41]. However, wing-removed and thus silent males of D. lummei were able to copulate with D. montana females, provided that pulse song with the montana-specific IPI was played back in mating assays [41]. In contrast, pulse song with the limmei-specific IPI had no “rescuing” effect [41]. QTL analysis of the IPI difference between two melanogaster relatives, D. simulans and D. sechellia, suggested cro in addition to maleless and fru as candidate loci for the diversification in this trait [25]. Molecular identification of cro by the present work will pave the way to the determination of potential changes in the song shaping function of this gene in evolution, thereby unveiling the mechanistic basis underlying precopulatory isolation in incipient speciation.

Material and methods

Drosophila stocks

Flies were reared on a cornmeal-yeast medium under a 12:12 light:dark cycle at 25 °C, except when specifically stated. The following strains were established or kept as lab stocks in our laboratory: the wild-type Canton-S, w1118, cro, elavC155 (elavGAL4), nSybGAL4, repoGAL4, Act5CGAL4 and UAS-mCD8::GFP. The following strains were obtained from the Bloomington stock center: Df(2R)Np5 (#3591), Df(2R)w45-30n (#4966), Df(2R)w73-2 (#6247), Df(2R)BSC408 (#24912), CamtaMI01580 (#32766), CamtaMI04570 (#37957), Camtaf05489 (#18874), CamtaMI01132A (#61689), CamtaMB05101 (#24606), UAS-Camta-RNAi (#35683, #40849), tubP>GAL80> (#38879, #38880) and tubP>stop>GAL80 (#38878). UAS-Camta+ (#F000702) strain was obtained from the Zurich ORFeome Project (FlyORF). Df(2R)ED1791 (#150124) and NP2631 (#104266) strains were obtained from Kyoto Stock Center. BAC-CH321-22B08 strain was obtained from BACPAC Resources Center (BPRC). VT40556 (#201129) strain was obtained from the Vienna Drosophila Resource Center. Otd-FLP was the kind gift from David J. Anderson (California Institute of Technology). fruGAL4, fruFLP, UAS>stop>mCD8::GFP, and p52a-GAL4 strains were the kind gifts from Barry Dickson (Janelia Farm). dsxGAL4 was the kind gift from Stephen F. Goodwin (University of Oxford).

Behavioral assays

The virgin males and females were collected at eclosion. Males were placed singly in food vials, while 10 females were placed together in single food vials. They were kept at 25 °C until being subjected to song recording, which was carried out on males aged 5–7 days after eclosion. The male was placed in a small chamber (0.8 cm in diameter, 0.3 cm in height) with a wild-type virgin female (3–5 days after eclosion). Courtship songs were recorded via a microphone (NR-23160; Knowles) placed beneath the chamber as described previously [43]. The sound signal from the microphone was amplified by a differential amplifier (DP-304; Warner Instruments) and recorded by an AD converter (ML826 PowerLab 2/26; AD Instruments). The maximal height of a song pulse was measured between the largest positive or negative peak and the baseline, which was defined as the mean output voltage of a 3-sec period that immediately preceded the rising phase of the relevant deflection (in contrast to the conventional definition of the amplitude of a pulse, which refers to the distance between the largest positive and negative peaks). To determine the number of cycles within a pulse, we set the slice level at 50% of the maximal height of each pulse unless otherwise specified, and if any deflections other than the primary (largest) peak exceeded the slice level, those deflections were counted as auxiliary peaks. When a song pulse had no additional peak that exceeded the slice level (other than the primary peak), it was classified as monocyclic. When a song pulse had one or more auxiliary peak(s), it was considered to be polycyclic. Song pulses in arbitrarily chosen segments of song records from 10 male flies of a given genotype were used to calculate the proportion of the polycyclic events. Our method to determine song pulse cycles was adapted from Rendahl et al. [44], where the slice level was set at 25% rather than 50%. The change from 25% to 50% will reduce false-positive counts due to the superposition of noise on a song pulse. Songs were typically recorded for 10 min from a given fly. To obtain IPI histograms and power spectra for song pulses, song records obtained as above were fed into a Digidata 1440A digitizer (Molecular Devices), followed by the analysis of relevant parameters using Clampfit 10.6 software (Molecular Devices). For fast Fourier transformation, digitized song records were treated at the sampling rate of 10,000 Hz, after filtering low frequency components at ~ 1 Hz and high frequency components at ~500 Hz.

Molecular cloning of the cro gene by plasmid rescue

The genomic DNA fragment near the cro locus was cloned by plasmid rescue [45] using the plasmid sequence contained in the BmΔw cro mutagenic P-element. Genomic DNA was purified from cro homozygous flies using DNeasy Blood & Tissue Kit (Qiagen, 69504). 1 μg genomic DNA was digested overnight with EcoRI (New England Biolabs, R3101) and circularized with the T4 DNA ligase at room temperature for 1 hour, then transformed into JM109 E. coli competent cells (Nippongene, 310–06246). The genomic fragment surrounding the site of BmΔw insertion was sequenced with a primer BmΔw-F1 (5’-ATTCAGTGCACGTTTG-3’) using ABI 3500 Genetic Analyzer (Applied Biosystems).

Inverse PCR

Inverse PCR was performed as previously described [46]. Genomic DNA from cro homozygous flies was cut with BamHI (New England Biolabs, R0136), diluted, and circularized with the T4 DNA ligase. The BmDw-F1 forward (5’-ATTCAGTGCACGTTTG-3’) and BmDw-R4-3 reverse (5’-TGTCCGTGGGGTTTGA-3’) primers were used to amplify the flanking DNA to the 3’ end of the BmΔw P-element. The PCR product thus obtained was directly sequenced with ABI3500 Genetic Analyzer (Applied Biosystems).

qPCR

qPCR was performed using a LightCycler 1.0 system (Roche). Total RNA was extracted from the head of 10 male adults of the indicated genotype using an RNeasy Mini Kit (Qiagen, 74104). To quantify Camta transcript expression levels, RNA was converted into cDNA using a ReverTra Ace qPCR RT kit (TOYOBO, FSQ-101). cDNA was mixed with SYBR Premix Ex Taq II (TAKARA, RR820S) and 5 pmol of both forward (5’-AGCCGACAGTTTTCCATCAC-3’) and reverse (5’-CACTCGCCATGCTTATCAAA-3’) primers. RpL32 (rp49) was amplified as an internal control using the primer pair 5’-AGATCGTGAAGAAGCGCACCAAG-3’ (forward) and 5’-CACCAGGAACTTCTTGAATCCGG-3’ (reverse). qPCR was conducted at 95 °C for 30 sec (initial denaturation), followed by 40 cycles of denaturation at 95 °C for 5 sec, annealing at 55 °C for 30 sec and elongation at 72 °C for 30 sec. Data processing was performed using LightCycler Software Ver. 3.5 (Roche).

Generation of the cro-GAL4 transgenic fly line

To obtain the cro-GAL4 driver, we employed recombinase-mediated cassette exchange (RMCE) [47] at an MiMIC insertion within the cro locus. The GAL4-containing plasmid vector pBS-KS-attP2-SA(1)-T2A-GAL4-Hsp70 was injected together with ΦC31 plasmid DNA into the embryos of flies bearing the Mi{MIC}04570 insertion in the cro locus. The recombinant transformant was signified by loss of the yellow+ selection marker, and the orientation of the GAL4 insertion was determined by genomic PCR using the PCR primers Camta F1 (5’-GCTTCGGAAATCATGCTGTA-3’) and GAL4 R103 (5’-CACTTGGGCTTCTCCTTGCT-3’).

Generation of anti-Camta antibodies

A rabbit polyclonal anti-Camta antibody was raised against a mixture of two 14-mer peptides, TVNNQATEAPNRSQ and YSASSDNSSQISDE, which were both encoded by exon 13 of the Camta-encoding gene (residues 1046–1059 and 1077–1090 of Camta (GenBank accession number ACL83079)), and was affinity purified.

Immunohistochemistry

The central nervous system (CNS) was dissected from flies in PBS, and fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde dissolved in PBS for 60 min. Immunostaining was carried out as described previously [16], using the following antibodies at the indicated dilutions: rabbit anti-GFP (Sigma, A6455; 1:500), mouse Anti-Brp (nc82) (DSHB, 1:10), guinea pig anti-FruMale [48] (1:500) and rabbit anti-Camta (this study, 1:200). The secondary antibodies used were as follows: Alexa488-conjugated goat anti-rabbit IgG (Thermo Fisher, A-11034) at 1:500, Alexa546-conjugated goat anti-mouse, rabbit or guinea pig IgG (Thermo Fisher, A-11003, A-11035 or A-11074) at 1:500, and Alexa647-conjugated goat anti-mouse IgG (Thermo Fisher, A32723) at 1:500. Stacks of optical sections were obtained with a Zeiss LSM 510 META confocal microscope and were processed with Image J software.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Generation of cro-GAL4.

Top: the exon-intron organization and the insertion points of the MIMIC (MI04570) and cro P-element (BmΔw) in the cro locus. Middle: a schematic illustrating the structure of MIMIC composed of Minos inverted repeats (MiL and MiR), splice acceptor sites (SA), inverted ΦC31 attP sites (attP), stop-start sequence (3xStop), fluorescent marker EGFP, polyA and visible marker yellow+. The pBS-KS-attP2-SA(1)-T2A-GAL4-Hsp70 plasmid vector used for replacing MIMIC with T2A-GAL4 via recombination at attP and attB. SD: splice donner site.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1008309.s001

(TIF)

S2 Fig. cro-GAL4 expression in the central nervous system.

(A-E) cro-GAL4 expression in the larval, pupal and adult stages. Panels A to E are duplicates of panels D to H of Fig 5, respectively, except that only GFP signals are shown here with no superposition of nc82 signals. (F-K) Visualization with GFP (green) of cells specified by the cro-GAL4 and fruFLP intersection in the brain (F-H) and VNC (I-K). An anti-Fru antibody (magenta) labels many of these cells.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1008309.s002

(TIF)

S3 Fig. OtdFLP effectively restricts fru-GAL4 expression to the brain.

fru-GAL4 is expressed in the brain regardless of whether the OtdFLP + tubP>GAL80> cassette is present (A) or absent (B), whereas its expression in the VNC is only detected in the absence of the cassette in flies that carry UAS-mCD8::GFP and fru-GAL4. The tissues were stained with an anti-GFP antibody for fru-GAL4 (green) and counterstained with nc82. Scale bar: 50 μm.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1008309.s003

(TIF)

S1 Table. Genotypes of flies used in this study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1008309.s004

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Numerical data underlying graphs.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1008309.s005

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We thank David Anderson, Barry Dickson, Stephen Goodwin, the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center, Drosophila Genetic Resource Center at Kyoto, Vienna Drosophila Stock Center, FlyORF, and BACPAC Resources Center for fly stocks.

References

  1. 1. Schluter D, Conte GL. Genetics and ecological speciation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2009; 106 Suppl. 1: 9955–9962. pmid:19528639
  2. 2. Davis AW, Roote J, Morley T, Sawamura K, Herrmann S, Ashburner M. Rescue of hybrid sterility in crosses between D. melanogaster and D. simulans. Nature. 1996; 380 (6570): 157–159. pmid:8600389
  3. 3. Grillet M, Everaerts C, Houot B, Ritchie MG, Cobb M, Ferveur J-F. Incipient speciation in Drosophila melanogaster involves chemical signals. Sci. Rep. 2012; 2: 224. pmid:22355738
  4. 4. Matsuo T, Sugaya S, Yasukawa J, Aigaki T, Fuyama Y. Odorant-binding proteins OBP57d and OBP57e affect taste perception and host-plant preference in Drosophila sechellia. PLoS Biol. 2007; 5 (5): e118. pmid:17456006
  5. 5. Krstic D, Boll W, Noll M. Sensory integration regulating male courtship behavior in Drosophila. PLoS ONE. 2009; 4: e4457. pmid:19214231
  6. 6. Spieth HT. Mating behavior within the genus Drosophila (Diptera). Bull. Am. Mus. Nat. Hist. 1952; 99: 399–522.
  7. 7. Billeter JC, Atallah J, Krupp JJ, Millar JG, Levine JD. Specialized cells tag sexual and species identity in Drosophila melanogaster. Nature. 2009; 461 (7266): 987–991. pmid:19829381
  8. 8. Kyriacou CP, Hall J C. Learning and memory mutants impair acoustic priming of mating behavior in Drosophila. Nature. 1984; 308 (5954): 62–65. pmid:21275188
  9. 9. Carson HL. Mate choice theory and the mode of selection in sexual populations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 2003; 100 (11): 6584–6587. pmid:12743377
  10. 10. Bastock M, Manning A. The courtship of Drosophila melanogaster. Behaviour. 1955; 8: 85–111.
  11. 11. Bennet-Clark HC, Ewing AW. The love song of the fruit fly. Sci. Am. 1970; 223(1): 85–90. pmid:5431830
  12. 12. von Schilcher F. A mutation which changes courtship song in Drosophila melanogaster. Behav. Genet. 1977; 7 (3): 251–259. pmid:405965
  13. 13. Clemens J, Coen P, Roemschied FA, Pereira T, Mazumder D, Aldarondo D, Pacheco D, Murthy M. Discovery of a song mode in Drosophila reveals hidden structure in the sensory and neural drivers of behavior. Curr. Biol. 2018; 28(15):2400–2412.e6. pmid:30057309
  14. 14. Yamamoto D, Jallon J-M, Komatsu A. Genetic dissection of sexual behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 1997; 42: 551–585. pmid:9017901
  15. 15. Kimura K-i, Hachiya T, Koganezawa M, Tazawa T, Yamamoto D. Fruitless and Doublesex coordinate to generate male-specific neurons that can initiate courtship. Neuron. 2008; 59: 759–769. pmid:18786359
  16. 16. Kohatsu S, Koganezawa M, Yamamoto D. Female contact activates male-specific interneurons that trigger stereotypic courtship behavior in Drosophila. Neuron. 2011; 69: 498–508. pmid:21315260
  17. 17. von Phillipsborn A, Liu T, Yu JY, Masser C, Bidaye SS, Dickson BJ. Neural control of Drosophila courtship song. Neuron; 2011 69: 509–522. pmid:21315261
  18. 18. Yamamoto D, Koganezawa M. Genes and circuits of courtship behaviour in Drosophila males. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 2013; 14: 681–692.
  19. 19. Shirangi TR, Wong AM, Truman JW, Stern DL. Doublesex regulates the connectivity of a neural circuit controlling Drosophila male courtship song. Dev. Cell. 2016; 37(6):533–544. pmid:27326931
  20. 20. Ewing AW. The neuromuscular basis of courtship song in Drosophila: the role of the direct and axillary wing muscles. J. Comp. Physiol. 1979;130: 87–93.
  21. 21. Smith LA, Peixoto AA, Kramer EM, Villella A, Hall JC. Courtship and visual defects of cacophony mutants reveal functional complexity of a calcium channel alpha1 subunit in Drosophila. Genetics. 1998; 149 (3):1407–1426. pmid:9649530
  22. 22. Kulkarni SJ, Steinlauf AF, Hall JC. The dissonance mutant of courtship song in Drosophila melanogaster. Isolation, behavior and cytogenetics. Genetics. 1988; 118 (2): 267–285. pmid:3129335
  23. 23. Rendahl KG, Gaukhshteyn N, Wheeler DA, Fry TA, Hall JC. Defects in courtship and vision caused by amino acid substitutions in a putative RNA-binding protein encoded by the no-on-transient A (nonA) gene of Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 1996; 16: 1511–1522. pmid:8778301
  24. 24. Yokokura T, Ueda R, Yamamoto D. Phenotypic and molecular characterization of croaker, a new mating behavior mutant of Drosophila melanogaster. Jpn. J. Genet. 1995; 70: 108–117.
  25. 25. Gleason JM, Jallon JM, Rouault JD, Ritchie MG. Quantitative trait loci for cuticular hydrocarbons associated with sexual isolation between Drosophila simulans and D. sechellia. Genetics. 2005; 171 (4): 1789–1798. pmid:16143629
  26. 26. Diao F, Ironfield H, Diao F, Shropshire WC, Ewer J, Marr E, Potter CJ, Landgraf M, White BH. Plug-and play genetic access to Drosophila cell types using exchangeable exon casettes. Cell Rep. 2015; 10: 1410–1421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2015.01.059, pmid:25732830.
  27. 27. Wheeler DA, Kulkarni SJ, Gailey DA, Hall JC. Spectral analysis of courtship songs in behavioral mutants of Drosophila melanogaster. Behav. Genet. 1989; 19: 503–528. pmid:2508613
  28. 28. Pavlou HJ, Goodwin SF. Courtship behavior in Drosophila melanogaster: towards a ‘courtship connectome’. Curr. Opn. Neurobiol. 2013; 23: 76–83.
  29. 29. Asahina K, Watanabe K, Duistermars BL, Hoopfer E, González CR, Edjólfsdóttir EA, Perona P, Anderson DJ. Tachykinin-expressing neurons control male-specific aggressive arousal in Drosophila. Cell. 2014; 156: 221–235. pmid:24439378
  30. 30. Manoli DS, Baker BS. Median bundle neurons coordinate behaviours during Drosophila male courtship. Nature. 2004; 430 (6999): 564–569. pmid:15282607
  31. 31. Pant P, Iqbal Z, Pandey BK, Sawant SV. Genome-wide comparative and evolutionary analysis of Calmodulin-Binding Transcription Activator (CAMTA) family in Gossypium species. Sci Rep. 2018; 8 (1): 5573. pmid:29615731
  32. 32. Schwartz RJ, Schneider MD. CAMTA in cardiac hypertrophy. Cell. 2006; 125 (3): 427–429. pmid:16678087
  33. 33. Bouche N, Scharlat A, Snedden W, Bouchez D, Fromm H. A novel family of Calmodulin-binding transcription activators in multicellular organisms. J. Biol. Chem. 2002; 277: 21851–21861. pmid:11925432
  34. 34. Yang T, Poovaiah BW. A Calmodulin-binding/CGCG box DNA-binding protein family involved in multiple signaling pathways in plants. J. Biol. Chem. 2002; 277: 45049–45058. pmid:12218065
  35. 35. Gong P, Han J, Reddig K, Li HS. A potential dimerization region of dCAMTA is critical for termination of fly visual response. J. Biol. Chem. 2007; 282 (29): 21253–21258. pmid:17537720
  36. 36. Han J, Gong P, Reddig K, Mitra M, Guo P, Li HS. The fly CAMTA transcription factor potentiates deactivation of rhodopsin, a G protein coupled light receptor. Cell. 2006; 127 (4): 847–858. pmid:17110341
  37. 37. Yu JY, Kanai MI, Demir E, Jefferis GSXE, Dickson BJ. Cellular organization of the neural circuit that drives Drosophila courtship behavior. Curr. Biol. 2010; 20: 1602–1614. pmid:20832315
  38. 38. Cande J, Namiki S, Qiu J, Korff W, Card GM, Shaevitz JW, Stern DL, Berman GJ. Optogenetic dissection of descending behavioral control in Drosophila. Elife. 2018; 7 pii: e34275. pmid:29943729
  39. 39. Namiki S, Dickinson MH, Wong AM, Korff W, Card GM. The functional organization of descending sensory-motor pathways in Drosophila. Elife. 2018; 7 pii: e34272. pmid:29943730
  40. 40. Ding Y, Lillvis JL, Cande J, Berman GJ, Arthur BJ, Long X, Xu M, Dickson BJ, Stern DL. Neural evolution of context-dependent fly song. Curr Biol. 2019 Apr 1;29(7):1089–1099.e7. pmid:30880014
  41. 41. Saarikettu M, Liimatainen JO, Hoikkala A. The role of male courtship song in species recognition in Drosophila montana. Behav. Genet. 2005; 35 (3): 257–263. pmid:15864441
  42. 42. Yukilevich R, Harvey T, Nguyen S, Kehlbeck J, Park A. The search for causal traits of speciation: divergent female mate preferences target male courtship song, not pheromones, in Drosophila athabasca species complex. Evolution. 2016; 70 (3): 526–542. pmid:26831347
  43. 43. Koganezawa M, Kimura K-i, Yamamoto D. The neural circuitry that functions as a switch for courtship versus aggression in Drosophila males. Curr. Biol. 2016; 26: 1395–1345. pmid:27185554
  44. 44. Rendahl KG, Jones KR, Kulkarni SJ, Bagully SH, Hall JC. The dissonance mutation at the no-on-transient-A locus of D. melanogaster: genetic control of courtship song and visual behaviors by a protein with putative RNA-binding motifs. J Neurosci. 1992;12(2):390–407. pmid:1740687
  45. 45. Hanahan D, Lane D, Lipsich L, Wigler M, Botchan M. Characteristics of an SV-40 plasmid recombinant and its movement into and out of the genome of a murine cell. Cell. 1980; 21: 127–139. pmid:6250708
  46. 46. Potter CJ, Luo L. Splinkerette PCR for mapping transposable elements in Drosophila. PLoS ONE. 2010; 5(4):e10168. pmid:20405015
  47. 47. Venken KJ, Schulze KL, Haelterman NA, Pan H, He Y, Evans-Holm M, Carlson JW, Levis RW, Spradling AC, Hoskins RA, Bellen HJ. MiMIC: a highly versatile transposon insertion resource for engineering Drosophila melanogaster genes. Nat. Methods. 2011; 8: 737–743. pmid:21985007
  48. 48. Sato K, Ito H, Yokoyama A, Toba G, Yamamoto D. Partial proteasomal degradation of Lola triggers the male-to-female switch of a dimorphic courtship circuit. Nat. Commun. 2019; 10:166. pmid:30635583