Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Self /other recognition and distinction in adolescents with anorexia nervosa: A pilot study using a double mirror paradigm

  • Nathalie Lavenne-Collot ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    nathalie.lavenne-collot@chu-brest.fr

    Affiliations Département de Psychiatrie de l’Enfant et de l’Adolescent, CHRU Brest, Brest, France, Université de Bretagne Occidentale, Brest, France, Laboratoire du traitement de l’information Médicale, Inserm U1101, Brest, France

  • Emilie Maubant,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation

    Affiliations Département de Psychiatrie de l’Enfant et de l’Adolescent, CHRU Brest, Brest, France, Université de Bretagne Occidentale, Brest, France

  • Stéphanie Déroulez,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Validation

    Affiliation Département de Psychiatrie de l’Enfant et de l’Adolescent, CHRU Brest, Brest, France

  • Guillaume Bronsard,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Département de Psychiatrie de l’Enfant et de l’Adolescent, CHRU Brest, Brest, France, Université de Bretagne Occidentale, Brest, France, Département de Sciences Humaines et Sociales, EA 7479, EA 3279 (CEReSS, AMU), Brest, France

  • Moritz Wehrmann,

    Roles Conceptualization, Software

    Affiliations Laboratoire de Physiologie de la Perception et de l’Action UMR 7152 CNRS, Collège de France, Paris, France, Bauhaus-Universität Weimar, Weimar, Germany

  • Michel Botbol,

    Roles Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Université de Bretagne Occidentale, Brest, France, Professeur Emérite de Psychiatrie de l’Enfant et de l’Adolescent, Université de Bretagne Occidentale, Brest, France

  • Alain Berthoz

    Roles Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Professeur Honoraire au Collège de France, Paris, France, Centre Interdisciplinaire de Biologie (CIRB), Paris, France

Abstract

Background

Abnormalities in body perception in patients affected by anorexia nervosa have been widely studied, but without explicit reference to their relationship to others and the social processes involved. Yet, there are a several arguments supporting impairments in interpersonal relationships in these patients. Notably, some evidence suggests that self/other distinction (SOD), the ability to distinguish one’s own body, actions and mental representations from those of others could be impaired. But research remains scarce in this area.

Material and methods

A single-centre, prospective pilot study was conducted to investigate, for the first time, self-recognition and SOD in seven adolescents with anorexia nervosa compared with matched healthy controls (HCs) using the "Alter Ego"TM double mirror paradigm. This innovative device allows the progressive morphing of one’s own face to that of another and vice versa between two subjects that interact on opposite sides of the device. Two judgement criteria were used: 1) M1: the threshold at which subjects start to recognize their own face during other-to-self morphing, and 2) M2: the threshold at which subjects start to recognize the other’s face during self-to-other morphing. In a second part, SOD was reassessed during five different sensorimotor tasks aimed at increasing body self-consciousness in participants with anorexia nervosa.

Results

The results showed that the participants with anorexia nervosa exhibited earlier self-recognition in the other-to-self sequence and delayed other-recognition in the self-to-other sequence. Furthermore, in contrast with that of HCs, the critical threshold for switching between self and other varied with the direction of morphing in anorexia nervosa participants. Finally, when participants with anorexia were seated in a chair with a backrest and footrest strengthening the median axis of their body, the self-recognition threshold (M1) increased significantly, approaching that of controls.

Conclusions

Although additional research is needed to replicate the results of this pilot study, it revealed the first behavioural evidence of altered SOD in individuals affected by anorexia nervosa through an embodied, semiecological face-recognition paradigm. The relationships between anomalies in body perception and alterations in interpersonal relationships are discussed within an integrative framework from phenomenology to neuroscience, and new research and therapeutic perspectives are presented.

1 Introduction

Anorexia nervosa (AN) is an increasingly common eating disorder that primarily affects adolescent girls and young women. Abnormalities in body perception and dysmorphophobia are key symptoms [1]. Over the past decade, a growing number of studies have attempted to understand the disturbed experience of the body in AN. Most studies have focused on abnormalities in body image [2], a multidimensional construct that encompasses not only perceptual representations of the body but also broader semantic, aesthetic, and emotional aspects [3]. Notably, there is a consensus that patients with AN overestimate their own visually perceived body size (for a review see [4]). However, more recent findings suggest that other levels of body representation, such as body schema or body consciousness, may also be affected [5]. The body schema, originally conceived by Head and Holmes [6], refers to a dynamic unconscious sensorimotor representation of the body, built on tactile, kinesthetic, visual, and labyrinthine inputs. It is elicited by action, whether imagined, anticipated and/or performed [7]. In the case of AN, distortions in multisensory integration processing that is fundamental to the acquisition and updating of body consciousness [8] have been demonstrated in many sensory modalities (for a review see [9]). Research has indeed shown somato-tactile distortions of height in the horizontal plane [1012], disturbances in proprioception and kinesthetic processing [13, 14], interoceptive [15] and extraroceptive [16] alterations in sensitivity and awareness, decreased multisensory integration [1719] as well as deficits in sensorimotor/proprioceptive memory [20].

Despite the great interest of this research, it has mainly focused on the representation of the physical body through evaluations of the body in relation to the environment or even to objects, but without explicit reference to others or to the social processes involved in relations between self and others. Consequently, even recent studies fail to elucidate the links between anomalies in body representation and other work from psychodynamic theories or cognitive sciences showing that anorexia is associated with alterations in socio-cognitive and interpersonal functioning [21]. Notably, theory of mind and emotional empathy may be impaired in anorexia, as well as emotional and social functioning [2225]. Interestingly, the phenomenological approach to anorexia provides an integrative and multidimensional conception of bodily self-consciousness, which does not leave aside the interpersonal and intersubjective aspects of the disorder that are proeminent in the clinical presentation and relationships with anorexic [26, 27]. Notably, several authors emphasize an attitude of over-objectification towards the body in patients with anorexia, consisting in adopting the other’s perspective from a third-person rather than first-person embodied perspective, as if their body doesn’t pertain to their self and a weakening of self-other boundaries [5]. Moreover, even an extensive literature in cognitive neuroscience emphasizes the role of bodily self-consciousness and multisensory integration processing in establishing a clear distinction between self and other [2831]. Consequently, exploring the relationship between bodily abnormalities and the self/other distinction in anorexia seems to be a promising avenue.

Self / Other distinction

In recent years, the concept of self/other distinction (SOD) has received growing interest [3235] and its study is increasingly relevant from a transosographical perspective in psychiatric disorders [36]. Despite a variety of terminology, including "differentiation”, "distinction”, "switching," or "agency" of self or other, all of the postulated processes seem to share a common characteristic of "controlling" shared representations between self and other [37, 38]. The ability to differentiate one’s own body, actions, and mental states from those of others is crucial for establishing relationships with others while maintaining a stable sense of self [8, 35, 39]. Such social experiences involve both an ability to identify with others and an ability to distinguish ourselves from others [40, 41]. In the absence of this capacity for SOD, confusion between self and other can occur: the experience of others can be confused as coming from the self (i.e., "altercentric" bias), or one can assume an understanding of the other’s mind based on one’s own experience (i.e., "egocentric" bias) [42, 43].

Disruptions in SOD have been previously demonstrated in a number of diseases or conditions such as borderline personality disorder [44], schizophrenia [45], Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) [35] and alexythmia [46]. Interestingly, patients with AN show increased levels of alexithymia [47], and there are numerous studies supporting the links between AN and ASD [48].

There are several arguments supporting SOD impairments in individuals with AN. First, pychodynamic works on the pathogenesis of the disorder argues that AN is frequently associated with the notion of "false self" [49], which is a common condition in early childhood in which children perceive their caregivers’ thoughts or desires as their own because they cannot distinguish them. Similarly, AN is also frequently associated with a failure in the separation-individuation line of development [5052]. In addition, findings from cognitive science have shown that AN is associated with an empathy deficit [53], which encompasses difficulty distinguishing between the respective perspectives of self and others and the ability to switch between them [54]. Moreover, authors have highlighted impairments in spatial cognition in AN, including predominant egocentricity during spatial performance tasks [55]. Notably, many studies support the relationship between altered spatial references, perspective changes, and social cognition or empathy [54, 5658], suggesting egocentricity also in SOD.

Some neurobiological findings further support SOD impairments in AN. In an RMI study, Sachdev explored the neural basis of self-body recognition in AN patients compared to healthy controls. When the two groups were compared in terms of differential activation with self versus non-self images, patients with AN showed no significant region activation with self images relative to baseline [59]. In addition, Vocks et al. exposed AN patients and control subjects to photographs of their own body and to the body of an unfamiliar other person [60]. In the AN group, visualization of their own body was associated with a significant decrease in a complex neural network. According to the authors, these results could reflect a body-related avoidance behavior. However, an alternative hypothesis could be a disturbance of self-recognition since this neural network is also involved in visual self-recognition [61]. Furthermore, the literature on alterations in social and affective touch in anorexia nervosa is particularly interesting for shedding light on the links between anomalies in body representation and social interactions. Indeed, touch is one of the earliest human experiences that evokes both the perception of the limits of one’s own body and a fundamental form of social interaction [62, 63]. Especially, learning to differentiate self from non-self begins reflexively in the womb [64, 65], and tactile cues play a crucial role in early development [66, 67] as well as in social communication and bonding [68]. While affective touch is perceived as most pleasurable in typically developing populations [69], research has shown that the hedonic value of touch is perceived as unpleasant in people with AN [7072] with neural hypersensitivity persisting even after remission [73]. Furthermore, with regard to the specific issue of self/other distinction, in an fMRI study using a task based on the comparison between self-touch versus other-touch, alterations in neural processing were demonstrated in participants with AN, with tactile sensations produced by the self rendered more similar to those produced by others [74].

To date, studies that have explored SOD generally used prerecorded static images or movies that progressively morph from one’s own face to another’s face and vice versa, both in healthy individuals [29, 75, 76] and in patients with mental disorders [44]. Notably, Hirot et al. [77] assessed self-recognition abilities in patients with AN via such a randomized self/other face morphing task using static images. They showed a significative difference with healthy subjects, with AN participants having more difficulty detecting facial changes and requiring images containing more ’’self’’ for self-recognition. The authors concluded to impairments in the recognition of one’s own face, a marker of self-awareness, in patients with AN. Furthermore, contrary to the previously suggested hypothesis of a predominant egocentricity in AN, these results instead support an altercentric predominance in the SOD, with a greater tendency to recognize the other rather than oneself.

However, it can be questioned whether static image recognition paradigms are sufficient for SOD assessment. Indeed, several studies have supported that the sense of one’s own body, variously termed “embodiment” [78] or bodily self-consciousness [79] is increasingly relevant for understanding clinical conditions such as AN [80, 81]. Therefore, studies of AN from an embodied perspective are urgently needed, as well as paradigms involving participants who are physically present to improve our understanding of AN physiopathology [82].

Here, for the first time, we examined self-recognition and SOD abilities in individuals with AN using a self-versus-other face identification task through a novel paradigm using the Double Mirror "Alter Ego"TM invented by the artist Moritz Werhmann.

This new paradigm developed by Berthoz and Thirioux allows us to specifically explore SOD under greater ecologically relevant conditions than the use of static images by merging the faces of two subjects physically facing each other and interacting on two sides of the mirror. It has been successfully used in several studies on self/other interactions in healthy subjects [83] and patients with psychiatric disorders, especially schizophrenia [45] and Autism Spectrum Disorders [84]. As shown in Fig 1, participants watch a double mirror in which a picture of their own face gradually transforms into the face of an unfamiliar other (self-to-other sequence) or vice versa (other-to-self direction) and indicate at which point they judge the morph to look more like their own face than the other person’s face.

thumbnail
Fig 1.

Experimental setting (Fig 1a + Fig 1b). Fig 1a. Picture of the double mirror Alter Ego designed by Moritz Wehrmann. The experiment took place in a completely darkened enclosed space in the research centre of the Brest Hospital. The mirror was installed on a square table. The AN patient and his matched control sat facing each other on either side of the double mirror. Fig 1b. Schematic representation of the individuals during the recognition task. Both participants wore a black cape covering the neck, the whole chest and the arms so that only the face was visible. They were asked to look each other straight in the eye and to focus only on the other person’s face. Height-adjustable chairs allowed strict alignment of the eyes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309548.g001

A mirror is a familiar and ecologically relevant everyday tool that plays a crucial role in several theoretical systems, from both developmental and psychodynamic perspectives. An ability for self-recognition in a mirror is indicative of an underlying self-concept and an important behavioural marker of higher-order consciousness [85, 86]. In particular, the ability to differentiate one’s own image from that of another in the specular image is considered to be a precursor of self-awareness reached at approximately 4 months old [87]. Moreover, Zazzo [88] described the way in which recognition of others (acquired at 8 months) far precedes self-recognition (acquired at approximately 2 years old) with the progressive awareness of one’s own body image. From a phenomenological perspective, the image reflected in the mirror also reveals the ambiguity of realizing one’s body as an object visible from the outside or by others, and yet being that object oneself—in other words, a tension between a primary embodied first person perspective and an internalized third-person perspective [89]. This intrapersonal tension also becomes an interpersonal one: while realizing that I am objectified or even reified by the gaze of the other, I must resist this gaze and (re)assert my own subjectivity, even if it is by objectifying the other in turn [90].

The objective of the present study was to examine self-recognition and SOD abilities in adolescents with AN compared to Healthy controls (HCs) by using the ability provided by this device to progressively change the identity of self and other in the mirror with changes in lighting on both sides of the device. This is a special kind of morphing using real actors, in contrast with the computerized methods usually used in such studies [29, 44, 75, 76].

Furthermore, in order to explore the relationships between body schema distorsions and impairments in social interaction and cognition, different sensorimotor conditions were applied to examine their effects on self-recognition and SOD.

Thus, by considering face recognition in a mirror as a key marker of self-consciousness [85, 86] but also the crucial first interface between self and other [91], our study aims to explore an interpersonal dimension of self-disturbance in AN, which is still lacking in the literature.

2 Material and methods

2.1. Participants

The study was conducted with seven individuals with AN (mean age: 15.6 years; 1 male, 6 females) matched by age and sex with seven unfamiliar Healthy control (HC) individuals. For logistical reasons related to the availability of the device, we were not able to recruit more participants at this stage of the research. However, given that small samples may be sufficient to show the presence of an effect but not for estimating the effect size [92, 93], this limitation is compatible with the hypotheses of this pilot study; specifically, that independent of the statistical representativeness of the study, AN individuals would exhibit differences in SOD compared to HCs on semiecological Double Mirror paradigm. Nevertheless, we successfully matched our participants with respect to age and age at testing.

Individuals with AN were recruited from Brest University Psychiatric Hospital in a unit specialized in the treatment of AN. None of the patients suffered from acute symptom exacerbations at the inclusion. They were all stabilized at recruitment and testing and not were taking any medication.

The inclusion criteria were (a) a diagnosis of AN made by a psychiatrist according to the DSM-V [94] and ICD-10 [95] criteria; (b) aged between 12 and 17 years old to ensure that a stable body schema had been acquired [96]. We excluded individuals with (a) a history of epilepsy, (b) claustrophobia, (c) achluophobia, or (d) the presence of distinctive signs or visual deficits requiring vision correction (eyeglasses or contact lenses).

The clinical characteristics of participants with anorexia are reported in Table 1.

thumbnail
Table 1. Clinical characteristics of participants with anorexia nervosa.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309548.t001

The HC individuals matched with the AN patients were recruited in the Brest area by oral communication with local schools via participants and staff working at the hospital. They were unfamiliar with individuals in the AN group to avoid confusion between self-identification and familiarity. They were determined to (1) be free of any significant developmental, neurological or psychiatric disorders based on a medical examination nor personal history of eating disorders (2) have a normal BMI greater than or equal to 18.5 to 24.9 kg/m^2(3) attend typical schooling for which their chronological age corresponded to their developmental age. Similar to the AN group, the exclusion criteria for the HCs were (1) a history of epilepsy, (2) claustrophobia, (3) achluophobia, and (4) the presence of distinctive signs or visual deficits requiring vision correction (eyeglasses or contact lenses).

The study protocol was approved by an ethical standards committee and performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants and their parents prior to their inclusion in the study.

2.2. Paradigm

In this experiment, we used a new double mirror paradigm based on the Alter Ego System, which consists of a semitransparent double mirror (70 cm × 50 cm × 0.4 cm; height×width×depth) with a set of computer-controlled white light-emitting diodes (LEDs) fixed on the mirror frame on both sides (Fig 1A).

These sets of LEDs can emit continuous lighting at different intensities, either separately (i.e., LEDs turned on for only one side of the mirror) or simultaneously (i.e., LEDs turned on for both sides of the mirror). Both the sampling switch between the two LED sets and the flicker frequency range (1–20 Hz) were controlled by a PC using E-Prime software. This system enables the generation of different self-face and other-face perceptual conditions when two individuals, A and B, are facing either side of the mirror (Fig 1B).

  • If the LEDs are turned on for subject A’s side, whereas the LEDs are turned off for subject B’s side, subject A can see his or her own face reflected in the mirror without seeing subject B’s face through the mirror. This perceptual condition is referred to by Thirioux [83] and Keromnes [45] as the self condition.
  • Using this same lighting mode, subject B can see subject A’s face through the mirror (through a transparent window) without seeing his or her own reflection. This perceptual condition is referred to as the other condition [45, 83].
  • When both sets of LEDs are on, the reflections of subject A’s and subject B’s faces merge in the mirror, making it potentially difficult for an individual to recognize his or her own face. The higher the light intensity is, the more visible the image of an individual in the mirror.

The experimental setting is described in Fig 1.

2.3. Protocol and task

2.3.1. Procedure.

The experimental procedure had a duration of approximately 90 minutes and was divided into 2 parts:

  • First, the neutral condition, lasted 15 minutes and consisted of a « back-and-forth » passage, i.e. TDCs moved progressively from self to other, then from other to self while, during the same period of time, participants with AN experienced the opposite condition, i.e. they moved from other to self, then from self to other;
  • The second part, called the sensorimotor stimulation condition, lasted 60 minutes and involved exposing participants to the same light intensity conditions as described in the neutral condition, i.e. a "back-and-forth » passage, but repeated 5 times with a different stimulus applied to the participants with AN during each passage.

Thus, the whole task consisted of 6 back-and-forth passages: one in neutral condition, then 5 with different successive sensory stimuli only for AN participants.

There was a 10-min pause between the first and second parts to allow attentional recovery.

First part: Neutral condition. This experimental protocol was inspired by the one previously used by Du Boisgueheneuc in Alzheimer’s disease patients, Keromnes and Tordjman in patients with schizophrenia [45] and Lavenne-Collot in patients with ASD [84]: the light intensity of the two LED sets were gradually and independently increased or decreased on the two sides of the mirror, such that the two individuals found themselves alternatively in the other condition or the self condition, as explained below.

Before starting the task, participants were seated on opposite sides of the device so that the eyes of both partners were strictly aligned in the mirror reflection. They were instructed not to make any body or facial movements (including facial expressions or grimaces) for the whole duration of the task.

At the beginning of the experiment, the patient was in the other condition, i.e., he starts by seeing the control subject’s face through the mirror without seeing his own face; then, the patient’s own image in the mirror becomes increasingly more apparent as a function of the light intensity.

Then, the patient is in the self condition, i.e., he begins to see his own face reflected in the mirror without seeing the control’s face; then, with changes in the light intensity, the control subject’s image in the mirror becomes increasingly more apparent.

At the same time, the control subject undergoes the same procedure, except that he starts in the self condition which transforms into the other condition.

Similar to Keromnes et al. [45], we called a “passage back and forth” the series of conditions that permitted each subject to return to the starting conditions after the conditions with identical light intensities was presented twice. Thus, one passage back and forth consists of the following sequence:

The simultaneous alterations in light intensity on each side of the mirror experienced by the AN patient and his matched control are presented in Fig 2A. Importantly, since modifications in the light intensity on one side of the mirror modify its reflective properties on both sides, we should consider the relative light intensity that is actually perceived by participants on each side, as shown in Fig 2B.

thumbnail
Fig 2.

Simultaneous alterations in light intensity on each side of the mirror over time (Fig 2a + 2b). Fig 2a. Light intensity over time.

  1. - At the beginning of the procedure, the light intensity was 100% for the HCs (mirror effect), whereas the light intensity was 0% for the AN patients (transparent window effect). -Then, the light intensity is progressively increased in 10% steps on the AN patient’s side while remaining at 100% for the HCs until 100% is reached on both sides.
  2. - During this first step, the AN patient’s image progressively appears and becomes increasingly fused with the HC’s image.
  3. - When both sides are at 100%, the light intensity is progressively decreased in 10% steps on the HC’s side, while it remains at 100% on the AN patient’s side.
  4. - During this step, the HC’s image progressively fades until its total disappearance when the light intensity drops to 0%.
  5. - Then, the reverse procedure is used to return to the initial configuration (100% on the HC’s side, i.e., a mirror effect for the HCs, and 0% on the AN patient’s side, i.e., transparent window effect for the AN patients).
  6. - Therefore, a condition with identical light intensities is presented twice to the participants during this sequence called one “passage back and forth”.

Fig 2b. Relative light intensity perceived by each participant over time. Relative light intensity perceived by the participants taking into account that modifications of light intensity on one side modifies its reflective properties on both sides.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309548.g002

After every change in light intensity, the participants were instructed to identify whether the presented image mostly corresponded to their own face or to the other’s face. Both subjects were asked a simple question: “who do you recognize most in the mirror?” The expected response was either “me” or “her/him” (to designate the other person), without any other alternative (i.e., there were no different answers and no answer was not permitted). The question was always addressed first to the individual with AN and then to the HC participant. Each stimulus was presented for 10 seconds each during a progressive sequence. The response time was unlimited. Once the answer was given by each participant, the next stimulus was presented. The type of response measured and recorded was a verbal response. The use of response button boxes was initially discussed as a way to control for bias in participants’ verbal responses. However, in addition to the difficulty of using manual buttons in totally dark lighting conditions, visual control might have been required to press the button, and thus, attention to the mirror image could have been disrupted or the movement could have induced a misalignment of the 2 faces. In addition, as in Keromnes [45], oral responses seemed to reassure these patients, as the task involved maintaining relationships with others and were more relevant in terms of identity reinforcement and assertive self-recognition.

Second part: Sensorimotor stimulation condition. In this second part, the same procedure was applied i.e. each stimulus of identical light intensity was presented twice to the participants during one passage back and forth as previously described in the neutral condition, but the postural or sensory stimulation conditions for the AN individuals were changed while the configuration for the HC individuals remained unchanged throughout the whole procedure. Indeed, our aim was to explore whether sensorimotor conditions reinforcing body image could improve the performance obtained by the AN participants. Accordingly, the tasks were used only for AN participants to investigate this compensatory effect, assuming that healthy control participants have intact bodily self-consciousness abilities (without the need for reinforcing supports)

As previously mentioned, body schema and self consciousness are both based on a multisensory integration process [8]. Therefore, we modified sensory inputs in participants with AN to assess their effect on mirror self-recognition and SOD. Five different tasks were administered, including different sensory modalities and locations, in agreement with psychomotor therapists, based on their knowledge of strategies used to reinforce body schema in usual care of patients with AN.

The objectives of these different tasks were: (1) to increase the awareness of each of the 2 hemi-bodies by successive weighting of its (1) right and (2) left sides, (3) to reinforce the median axis of the body by providing increased postural support at the level of both the back and the pelvis, (4) to stimulate the vestibular system with a cushion generating an unstable position, and (5) to provide intensive somatosensory stimulation with a neoprene jacket.

Participants were tested successively on each of these distinct tasks that were administered always in the same order:

  1. Right hemi-body weighting (RHB) condition: Weights of 75 grams were placed around the AN patient’s right wrist and right ankle
  2. Left hemi-body weighting (LHB) condition: Weights of 75 grams were placed around the patient’s left wrist and left ankle
  3. Back- and Foot- Rest (BFR) condition: Patients with AN were no longer seated on a stool but in a chair with a backrest and a step was placed under their feet to allow pelvic flexion
  4. Unstable Cushion (UC) condition: Patients with AN were sitting on an unstable cushion installed on the stool
  5. Neoprene (NP) condition: Patients wore a neoprene jacket

Debriefing occurred at the end of the experiment during an informal and friendly period of time that did not include data collection. A snack with cakes and drinks was offered during this period. The participants were asked about their feelings regarding the task and their awareness about differences in stimulation, asked whether they found face recognition and discrimination difficult, and whether they were familiar with this type of morphing (as some teenagers regularly use morphing applications).

2.3.2. Data analysis.

The main outcome was the light intensity levels at which critical changes in self/other identification occurred:

  1. Level M1 was the critical perceptual threshold corresponding to the first time the individual recognized himself when his own image progressively appeared in the mirror during the other-to-self morphing sequence.
  2. Conversely, level M2 was the critical perceptual threshold corresponding to the first time the individual recognized the other’s image in the mirror when this image progressively appeared in the mirror during the self-to-other morphing sequence.

To summarize, the lower the M1 or M2 levels were, the larger the proportion of self was in the image (and the smaller the proportion of the other), which reflected a difficulty for the individual to uncouple from his own image.

Determination of M1 and M2 levels as well as a synthesis of the experimental procedure are shown in Fig 3.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Synthesis of the experimental procedure during one “passage back and forth”.

During the task, the image the AN patient sees undergoes a morphing from other to self and then from self to the other. Simultaneously, the image the HC sees undergoes a morphing from self to other and then from other to self. The threshold M1 corresponds to the light intensity at which the perceptual shift from other to self occurs. The M2 threshold corresponds to the light intensity at which the perceptual shift from self to other occurs. The procedure is then repeated a second time. Thus, for each subject, 2 M1 and 2 M2 thresholds are obtained.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309548.g003

2.3.3. Statistical analysis.

After each step (change in light intensity), the participant’s verbal responses were recorded. The analysis of the main outcome variable was conducted by comparing the M1/M2 levels (expressed as percentage of light intensity) between individuals with AN and HCs. As this variable was not normally distributed, the nonparametric one-tailed Mann–Whitney test was used to compare M1 and M2 levels between the two groups.

The effects of differents sensorimotor tasks on the M1 and M2 levels were assessed by using the non-parametric Wilcoxon test to compare for the AN patients the results obtained in the neutral condition and the results obtained in the 5 sensorimotor conditions (RHB, LHB, BFR, UC, NP).

3 Results

The comparison of the results for the self/other distinction task in the different conditions between individuals with AN and HCs are presented in Table 2.

thumbnail
Table 2. Comparison of the results for the recognition task between individuals with AN (N  =  7) and Healthy controls (N  =  7) in Neutral (Neu), Right hemi-body weighting (RHB), Left hemi-body weighting (LHB), Back- and Foot- Rest (BFR), Unstable Cushion (UC) and Neoprene (NP) conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309548.t003

3.1. Neutral condition

In neutral condition, M1 levels were significantly lower in the individuals with AN than in the HCs (p = 0.001). This indicated that the AN individuals showed an “earlier” self -recognition in the other condition (i.e., when one’s own image was gradually appearing in the mirror in the other-to-self direction of the morphing).

Similarly, M2 levels were significantly lower in the AN individuals than in the HCs (p = 0.02). This indicated that the AN individuals showed a delay in recognizing the other in the self condition (i.e., when the image of the other was gradually appearing in the mirror in the self-to-other direction of the morphing).

The Wilcoxon paired-samples test comparing M1 and M2 showed that M2 levels were significantly higher than M1 levels in individuals with AN (p = 0.02). Conversely, there was no significant difference between M1 and M2 levels in HCs (p = 0.09). This indicated that, in the HCs, the critical perceptual threshold corresponding to the ability to recognize self and other was the same regardless of the direction of morphing. In other words, the critical threshold for switching between self and other appeared when viewing morphed faces that contained the same proportion of facial features of self and other, regardless of whether it was the other condition or self condition. In contrast, in the individuals with AN, this perceptual threshold varied depending on the direction of the morphing, and the shift from self to other or from other to self did not occur at the same threshold. Indeed, the AN group needed a smaller proportion of self (image of themselves) to recognize their own face in the other-to-self direction versus the self-to-other direction.

Comparison of M1 and M2 levels in ANs and HCs are presented in Fig 4 and a synthesis of M1 and M2 distribution levels in neutral condition is presented in Fig 5.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Comparison of M1 and M2 levels in HCs and ANs in neutral condition.

This figure indicates that, in the HCs, the critical perceptual threshold corresponding to the ability to recognize self and other was the same regardless of the direction of morphing. In contrast, in the individuals with AN, this perceptual threshold varied depending on the direction of the morphing, and the shift from self to other or from other to self did not occur at the same threshold.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309548.g004

thumbnail
Fig 5. Distribution of M1 and M2 in AN and HC in neutral condition.

Two points are superimposed.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309548.g005

3.2. Sensorimotor stimulation condition

When the AN individuals were installed in the LHB, RHB, UC and NP conditions, M1 levels were significantly lower in the AN individuals than in the HCs. This indicated that, in all those conditions, AN individuals showed an earlier self- recognition in the other condition (i.e., when one’s own image was gradually appearing in the mirror in the other-to-self direction of the morphing).

Conversely, there was no significative difference on M1 levels in ANs compared to HCs in the BFR condition. This indicated that, when AN individuals were seated in a chair with a backrest and a support under their feet, there was no significant difference in self-recognition levels compared to HCs. Distribution plots of M1 thresholds for ANs and HCs in the different conditions are presented in Fig 6.

thumbnail
Fig 6.

Distribution plots of M1 levels in AN and HC in Neutral (Neu), Right hemi-body weighting (RHB), Left hemi-body weighting (LHB), Back- and Foot- Rest (BFR), Unstable Cushion (UC) and Neoprene (NP) conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309548.g006

When the AN individuals were installed in the LHB, RHB, UC and NP conditions, M2 levels were significantly lower in the AN individuals than in the HCs. This indicated that, in all those conditions, AN individuals showed a delay in recognizing the other in the self condition (i.e., when the image of the other was gradually appearing in the mirror in the self-to-other direction of the morphing).

Conversely, there was no significant difference in M2 levels between ANs and HCs in the BFR condition. This indicated that, when AN individuals were in the BFR condition, there was no significant difference in other- recognition level compared to HCs.

Distribution plots of M2 levels in different conditions for individuals with AN and HCs are presented in Fig 7.

thumbnail
Fig 7.

Distribution plots of M2 levels in AN and HC in Neutral (Neu), Right hemi-body weighting (RHB), Left hemi-body weighting (LHB), Back- and Foot- Rest (BFR), Unstable Cushion (UC) and Néoprene (NP) conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309548.g007

To summarize, in the AN patients, M1 and M2 levels were significantly lower than in HCs in neutral, LHB, RHB, UC, and NP conditions, indicating that patients with AN recognized their own image earlier when it appeared in the mirror in the other condition and that they recognized the other person’s image later when the other was gradually appearing in the mirror in the self condition.

Conversely, there was no difference between ANs and HCs on the M1 and M2 levels when the AN individuals were in the BFR condition.

3.3. Comparison between neutral and sensorimotor conditions

In the AN group, M1 levels were significantly higher in the BFR than in the neutral condition. Conversely, there were no significative differences on M1 level between neutral and LHB, RHB, UC and NP conditions.

In the AN group, M2 levels were significantly lower in the LHB, RHB, UC, and NP conditions than in the neutral condition. Conversely, there was no significant difference on M2 levels in the BFR condition compared with the neutral condition

These results indicated that when the recognition task was associated with the BFR condition versus the neutral condition, AN participants showed a delay in self recognition but no significant difference in other recognition. Conversely, in ANs, there was a delay in recognition of the other when the visual recognition was associated with the LHB, RHB, UC, NP conditions compared to the neutral condition, but no significant difference in self recognition.

4 Discussion

Our results are based on a very small sample; therefore, the preliminary nature of this study must be taken into account when analysing and discussing the results.

4.1. Self recognition in the mirror

4.1.1. Self- consciousness.

The results of the present study support that AN individuals show a difference in mirror face recognition thresholds compared to HCs on the double mirror task. These results are consistent with previous research showing that self-recognition abilities may be impaired in AN [59, 77]. Furthermore, in line with previous research showing the value of the double mirror device in studying bodily self-consciousness [46, 97], our results support the notion that self-disorder is an important dimension of AN [98, 99] and, in particular, that the embodied aspects of the self could be disturbed [5, 100].

4.1.2. Comparison with previous research using static images.

Our results showed that the AN individuals required images that contained less proportion of self than controls to recognize themselves in the reflected image, regardless of the direction of the morphing (i.e., in the "self" or "other" conditions). These results contrast with those found by Hirot et al. [77] during a self/other morphing task using static images that showed that patients required images containing more ’’self’’ to recognize themselves. Several arguments support this discrepancy: First, (i) contrary to photo, looking at ourselves in the mirror gives us access to our own image, besides the proprioceptive, tactile and motor sensory cues that are necessary for the representation of one’s own face [101]; (ii) studies have found that mirror self-recognition emerges prior to photo self-recognition [102]; (iii) different neural responses have been found when comparing mirror and photo self-processing [103] and; (iv) preserved self-face recognition on photographs despite incapacity in mirrors has been described in some neurological patients showing a kind of agnosia for recognition of their own reflected image [104]. Moreover, our experimental design differs from that of Hirot’s study because they used an aleatory morphing protocol thought we presented the morphs incrementally from 0% self/other to 100% self/other in two directions separately.

4.2. Egocentricity in self/other distinction

4.2.1. Implications regarding spatial cognition: Privileged egocentric reference frame.

The results obtained in the present study showing earlier self-recognition and delayed other recognition in the AN individuals, compared to the HCs, could be interpreted as a difficulty to inhibit their own perspective during the face recognition task and adopt the reference frame of the other, resulting in a privileged use of the egocentric reference frame.

Indeed, unlike when one is faced with a simple photograph, the mirror requires processes of perspective shifting and spatial transformations beyond pure self-recognition. In particular, self-recognition in the mirror requires matching one’s sensorimotor experience (1st person perspective) with the object seen in the mirror (3rd person perspective), thereby identifying the "I" with the "me" and representing that self as an object to others and to oneself [103]. Moreover, the face-to-face postural configuration of both subjects during the Alter Ego double mirror task requires a 180° mental rotation of one’s own body. In particular, Thirioux et al. [83] emphasized the role of visuospatial abilities on self-recognition and SOD within this task. Thus, our results are consistent with previous studies showing alterations in spatial cognition in ANs and, in particular, a privileged use of the egocentric frame of reference during spatial performance tasks [14, 55]. Moreover, given the implications of impaired spatial references for social cognition and perspective shifting or empathy [54, 5658], this raises the question of the ability of patients with AN to infer mental states other than their own. To date, the nature of the links between alterations in body schema and alterations in spatial cognition remains unknown [14]: is it the alteration of the body signal that may participate in the disruption of spatial cognition (bottom up effect)? Or is it the alteration of spatial cognition that may participate in the alteration of body schema (top down effect)? Overall, analysis of the results under sensorimotor conditions tends to support the second hypothesis (see below).

4.2.2. Implications for multisensory processing: Overinclusion of the other’s face.

Our results showed that the AN individuals, compared to the HCs, judged the morphs to be more like the self in both directions of morphing. These findings can be interpreted as a greater level of assimilation of the other’s face in the representation of one’s own face among the AN individuals than the HCs. Such misattribution of others’ facial features to oneself has been reproduced in nonclinical populations through simple psychophysical manipulations in a procedure involving sensory processing called interpersonal multisensory stimulation [29]. Indeed, studies using body ownership illusions have shown that, under certain conditions, the sense of self can be manipulated to include a fake or another person’s body part, for instance, the hand [105], face [101, 106, 107] or whole body [108].

The double mirror task involves a simultaneous visual perceptual conflict between the visual input (“I see the other”) and the proprioceptive experience (“I expect to see myself in the mirror”). This conflict is reinforced by the everyday experience we may have developed that, when we look in the mirror, we see ourselves and not another person. As specified by Thirioux [83], here we studied the effect of a unisensory (visual) conflict, whereas previous studies have typically studied the effect caused by multisensory (especially visual-tactile) illusions [29].

Several studies have revealed abnormalities in the multisensory integration process in patients with anorexia nervosa [9]. In particular, people with anorexia nervosa have been found to exhibit greater sensitivity to the Rubber Hand Illusion [105], indicative of a greater susceptibility to appropriating and embodying an object external to their own body, i.e. a more plastic bodily self [13, 109]. Yet conflicting results have shown similar levels of sensitivity in full-body illusion tasks compared to controls [110, 111], but only when a highly emotionally valenced body part, such as the abdomen, was stimulated to promote the illusion [9].To our knowledge, this greater susceptibility to the embodiment illusion for body parts (contrasting with full-body) has never been explored at face level in individuals with AN, as assessed through paradigms such as the “enfacement illusion” [106, 112]. However, both body plasticity and face representation have been shown to rely on the same multisensory integration processes [8, 101]. Thus, the results of the present study are consistent with the generalization to the face of a greater tendency to the embodiment illusion for body parts in AN individuals.

Moreover, our results indicated that people with AN continued to recognize themselves in the mirror despite the increase in the proportion of the other person’s face in the image, regardless of the direction of the morphing. These results may indicate that, in the AN individuals, the proprioceptive system would be less vulnerable to bias originating from visual information (i.e., AN individuals depend more heavily on proprioceptive information than on visual information when incongruent). This finding is in line with previous studies that pointed out an overreliance on proprioception in the presence of competing signals from other modalities [17]. Moreover, they are also consistent with reduced integration of visual and proprioceptive information in AN, also supported by the ubiquitous clinical finding in these patients that their visual perception of the body, especially in a mirror, does not correct their distorted body image [113].

4.2.3. Comparison between the self and other conditions: Imbalance mechanisms of projection / simulation.

Our results showed that the AN individuals, compared to the HCs, judged the morphs to be more like the self in both directions of morphing. Importantly, the comparison between self and other conditions shows that, unlike the HCs, the AN individuals switched from other to self and from self to other at different thresholds. This discrepancy indicated that the confusion between self and other may have been determined by a unidirectional overinclusion of the other’s attributes by the AN individuals. Conversely, the HCs may have exhibited compensatory mechanisms with bidirectional self- and other-facial feature attribution. i.e., they projected their facial features onto the other’s face as much as they introjected their partner’s facial features onto their own face. This understanding is supported by Thirioux’s findings using a double mirror in healthy subjects [83] and human interpersonal understanding based on balanced mechanisms of self-projection and simulation [41, 114]. Indeed, as noted by Freud “the boundaries of the self are not constant” [115]. That is, both processes also transformed the margins between the self and the other, either by externalizing facial features of the self into the other (projection) or by alienating facial features that do not belong to the self but to the other (introjection) [116]. Thus, our findings may be consistent with psychoanalysts who described anorexia as a failure of identification and a pathology of object relations [117] particularly marked by deficiencies in the balanced introjection/projection mechanisms that are necessary to establish a sense of identity, leading to characteristic forms of pathological projective identification often evident in these patients [118].

4.2.4. Switching between the abstracts of self and other.

Our experimental design requires switching from self to other and from other to self as the morphing gradually shifts from 0% to 100% between self and other and vice versa. Thus, the double mirror Alter Ego task is consistent with recent conceptualizations of SOD as being achieved through the ability to switch between representations of self and other and to inhibit the representation that is not relevant in a given situation [119]. In our study, differences in M1 and M2 thresholds between the AN individuals and HCs were consistent with AN being associated with an inadequate control of the self/other control or switch [37].

Another hypothesis explaining our results is that anorexic participants may have difficulties matching their own actions with their sensory inputs, i.e. difficulties in predicting the sensory outcome of their own action. This hypothesis is consistent with the theory of active inference, which postulates that the brain uses an internal model to continuously infer and control external or internal signals associated with the body [120, 121]. This theoretical framework is attracting growing interest in understanding alterations in sense of self [65], particularly in AN [122].

Indeed, although our task does not involve active movements, since participants were instructed not to move, we cannot exclude minimal movements such as eye blinking or respiratory movements. In any case, the double mirror task involves motor imagery, especially mental rotation [83] which implies predicting the sensory consequences of the imagined movement [123]. Usually, the brain weights and selects the most informative signals it receives to minimize prediction error and reduce surprise, notably by attenuating self-generated percepts that are highly predictable [124] through a copy of the motor signal called efference copying [125]. In AN, subjects have been shown to rely on incorrect predictions, i.e. on the unverified representation of their own body. In particular, some authors have highlighted a lack of integration of interoceptive and exteroceptive signals leading to an allocentric lock-in state, where body memory is not updated according to new sensory evidence relating to the body’s actual shape [122, 126]. Interestingly, with regard to the distinction between self and others, prediction errors have been demonstrated at the tactile level in subjects with AN, the lack of neural attenuation of tactile sensations produced by self making them more similar to those produced by others [74]. This finding is particularly interesting considering the crucial role of tactile cues in the construction of self-consciousness from fetal life [63, 66, 67].

4.3. Impact of sensorimotor tasks on self-recognition and SOD

4.3.1. Back and foot support condition.

Comparison between neutral and BFR conditions. Our results showed a significant increase in the M1 threshold in the BFR condition compared to the neutral condition. This indicated that, when they were in the BFR condition, the AN participants showed a delayed self-recognition, i.e., they continued to perceive the other longer while their own image was gradually appearing.

Back support is an essential early developmental modality for the construction of one’s identity, and in particular one’s body. Indeed, during fetal development, the uterine wall provides the primary back support that allows for the development of oral activities (swallowing, etc.). Even after birth, back support remains fundamental, especially in the sensorimotor model developed by Bullinger [127] that argues that postural control and tonic-sensory balance allow infants to interact with their environment and develop social involvement [128]. Notably, in a pilot study, Rahme et al. showed a significant increase in social attention, in the back support condition compared to no back support in children with neurodevelopmental disorders [129]. Similarly, the results of our study are consistent with the idea that the BFR condition may have helped the AN individuals to be more other-centered.

Comparison between AN and HC groups. In contrast to the results obtained in the neutral condition, when the AN participants were settled in the BFR condition, there was no more significative difference between the HC and AN groups. These results suggest -that the implementation of postural supports against the medial axis would have compensated for the alterations in self-recognition and SOD in the AN participants.

4.3.2. Other sensorimotor conditions.

As far as other sensorimotor conditions are concerned, both left and right hemibodies ballasting, unstable cushion and neorprene conditions had a greater effect on M2 threshold than on M1, including a decrease in M2 threshold. This indicates that these conditions had no significant effect on self-recognition, but were significantly associated with delayed other-recognition, suggesting that they may have helped the AN individuals to be more self-centered. These results are in line with previous research using double mirror in schizophrenia patients that showed that tactile and kinesthetic tasks helped to be more self-centered [45].

Furthermore, our results did not show any significant difference regarding the right- or left-lateralized tasks, which did not support a preferential input lateralization effect on face recognition and SOD thresholds in the AN individuals.

Regarding the Neoprene condition, previous research conducted in a single case study conducted by Grunwald showed that intensive somatosensory stimulation with a neoprene diving suit led to improved body image in the AN participants [130]. However, the neoprene suit was worn for one hour three times a day whereas, in our study, this condition may have been applied for too short a time to induce a change in self-recognition patterns.

4.4. Implications for AN comprehension: what’s new?

4.4.1. Relationship between bodily self-consciousness and socio cognitive functioning.

In summary, our results highlight: (i) disturbances in mirror self-recognition suggesting bodily self-consciousness impairments in AN; (ii) an egocentric bias in self/other disctinction found in AN participants compared to HCs and; (iii) changes in self-recognition and SOD thresholds were observed when sensorimotor or postural conditions were modified in individuals with AN.

Although our preliminary results must be put into perspective given the small sample size, they support the hypothesis that more fragile bodily self-consciousness in AN participants may impair self-referential processes (bottom up effect). This hypothesis is consistent with the results obtained by Thirioux [83] who showed an alteration of self-referential processes by inducing self-fragility through self/other fusion via the double mirror in healthy subjects.

4.4.2. More fragile median body axis.

Our results support fragilities of the body schema at the level of the median axis in AN. This hypothesis is consistent with clinical observations showing, in these patients, a misalignment of the whole body, especially with respect to this axis, a restricted breathing pattern coupled with muscle rigidity having a major negative impact on body stability [131], problems with stabilization of the back and pelvis in a neutral position and difficulties in performing pelvic movements [132] as well as gait abnormalities with biomechanical differences primarily in the pelvis and hip [133]. Interestingly, while studies on the effects of body therapies aimed at increasing body-awarness (often referred to as ‘body awareness therapies’) are still rare, the authors of a recent study on the lived body in patients with anorexia found that by allowing themselves to lean on the seat and back of the chair (during physiotherapy consultations), instead of perching on the edge of the chair, anorexic subjects felt more attentive and present in the here and now, indicating an increase in self-consciousness through the addition of medial postural support, consistent with our findings [134].

Furthermore, our results did not find a preferential effect of lateralized tasks contrasting with previous research supporting similarities found in AN patients with hemineglect patients [135] but with divergent results, just as the existence of a hemispheric predominance is still debated [82, 135]. More than a hemineglect similar to the one found in neurological patients, our results may raise the hypothesis of a functional cleavage or split at the level of the median axis that is supposed to connect the right and left parts of the body.

4.4.3. Further therapeutic and research implications.

As they highlight the prominence of abnormalities in body self-consciousness in understanding alterations in social-cognitive and interpersonal functioning in AN, our results also support the value of physical treatments in this condition. In particular, our results support that psychomotor therapy focused on strengthening and stabilizing the body axis could improve self-awareness, which could then have training effects on social-cognitive functioning.

In addition, the fragilities found in the median axis of the body raise the hypothesis of disturbances in the very early stages of body construction in AN patients, as observed in other neurodevelopmental disorders [127, 129]. Therefore, it would be interesting to look for possible early signs of vulnerability in the sensorimotor domain, following the example of retrospective studies using home movies performed for such disorders [136].

Finally, using the same protocol respectively in patients with schizophrenia and also ASD versus healthy controls, Keromnes and Tordjman [45] and Lavenne-Collot [84] found similar results to the present study (i.e., earlier recognition of self and later recognition of others). Interestingly, given the overlap in symptomatology between ASD and AN, it would have been interesting to collect participants’ autism spectrum quotient (AQ) scores, but this goes beyond the scope of this preliminary study. Overall, these results underline the value of considering SOD disturbances as a transdiagnostic dimension shared with other psychiatric or neurodevelopmental disorders and supports the interest of further research using the Alter Ego in a double mirror at the diagnostic but also therapeutic level.

Limitations and future directions

Some limitations of the study should be recognized. First, this is a preliminary study conducted with very small samples. Future studies are therefore needed to replicate the results in larger, samples.

Moreover, the self–other facial morphing task used in this study taps into perceptual SOD, that is, the capacity to identify one’s own body (here, one’s face) and to distinguish it from others. Therefore, it raises the question of whether the findings from the present study could be generalized to other domains of SOD. Although perceptual SOD and mental-state SOD should not be equated, evidence shows that they may be related [119] and that SOD may operate in a domain-general rather than domain-specific manner [137].

Another limitation of this study is that the differences between AN individuals and HCs may have occurred at the attentional or information processing level, i.e., upstream of the SOD process. Notably, our results could have been influenced by the cognitive functioning typically described in AN, including a deficit in cognitive flexibility, although these alterations showed contrary results in the literature (for a review see [138]). However, if a deficit in cognitive flexibility could account for the delay observed in the ability to switch from self to other that was found in the self condition, a similar delay should have been observed in the other condition, which is contrary to our results. Similarly, we don’t think our results can not be explained by an attentional or emotional bias related to the fact that subjects with AN dislike seing their own face in the mirror (i.e. they would have avoided or be more sensitive to seeing it). Indeed, if this hypothesis were correct, this tendency should have been the same regardless of the direction of morphing.this tendency should have been the same regardless of the direction of morphing.

In addition, patients with AN typically exhibit a strong desire for control that results in extreme organization and planning [139]. Given their usual perfectionism and fear of making mistakes, one might have expected a later self-recognition (similar to Hirot’s [77] results), i.e., the presented images should have contained more of their own face for participants to be more certain before responding. Interestingly, again, our results are opposite.

Furthermore, in the second part of the study, the postural and sensorimotor tasks were only applied to participants with AN. Therefore, the effects of these tasks in healthy subjects remained unknown. However, here, the purpose of our study was (i) to explore the relationship between body schema abnormalities and SOD abilities only in AN individuals (ii) to explore whether sensorimotor conditions reinforcing body image could improve the performance obtained by the AN participants. Therefore, the tasks were used only for patients to examine this compensatory effect compared to healthy controls.

Furthermore, we do not believe that the results obtained for the sensorimotor tasks can be explained by a distraction effect caused by the stimulation. Indeed, in this case, the distraction effect should have been observed in a similar way whatever the direction of morphing for the same stimulus. Moreover, the distraction effect should have been greater for the more unusual sensorimotor tasks (such as sitting on an unstable cushion). However, it is the effect of the BFR condition (which is not the most unusual) that emerges significantly from our results.

As far as experimental setting is concerned, there might be a bias of participants’ oral responses influencing each other. To decrease this possible bias in the individuals with AN, these participants were consistently asked to respond first during the task. Aleatory variations in light intensity rather than progressive linear changes were initially discussed to control for possible habituation bias in participants. However, abrupt changes in light intensity and the reflected images could have been stressful for the participants. Moreover, in line with other studies [29, 75, 76], we presented the morphs incrementally from 0% self/other to 100% self/other in the two directions separately. This allowed us to differentiate the morphing directions and disentangle the two types of self/other confusion (i.e., egocentric bias and altercentric bias) and the critical thresholds for switching between self and other.

Similarly, the order of the presentation of the task was not randomized, which could have led to carry-over effects. However, there was no significant time effect on the results of the recognition task in the individuals with AN or the HCs, which allowed us to reduce the possible carryover and learning effects of the task.

Conclusions

Although further research is needed to replicate these results, this study uncovered novel findings showing the first behavioural evidence of impaired SOD in individuals with AN through an embodied face-recognition paradigm generating a self–other face merging illusory effect in ecologically relevant conditions, i.e., when two individuals are physically facing each other and interacting.

This study is a first step in exploring the links between, on the one hand, body representation and sense of ownership, and, on the other hand, alterations in social interactions and cognitions that have unduly always been explored separately in the literature on AN.

Finally, the double mirror Alter Ego may also offer new therapeutic perspectives for AN based on a potential remediation pathway using double mirrors by practising control of representations of self and others and simultaneously improving the sense of body ownership, supporting self-differentiation, and correcting the poor self-perception that plays a causal role in the development, persistence, and relapse of AN.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the youth who participated in the study as well as Pierre Ailliot for the statistical analysis.

References

  1. 1. Treasure J, Claudino AM, Zucker N. Eating disorders. Lancet. 2010;375(9714):583–593.
  2. 2. Cash TF, Deagle EA 3rd. The nature and extent of body-image disturbances in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: a meta-analysis. Int J Eat Disord. 1997;22(2):107–125. pmid:9261648
  3. 3. Gallagher S. Body Image and Body Schema: A Conceptual Clarification. J Mind Behav. 1986; 7 (4): 541–554.
  4. 4. Mölbert SC, Klein L, Thaler A, et al. Depictive and metric body size estimation in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2017;57:21–31. pmid:28818670
  5. 5. Legrand D. Subjective and physical dimensions of bodily self-consciousness, and their dis-integration in anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(3):726–737. pmid:19778543
  6. 6. Head H, Holmes HG. Sensory Disturbances from Cerebral Lesions. Brain. 1911–1912; 34: 102–254.
  7. 7. de Vignemont F. Body schema and body image—pros and cons. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(3):669–680. pmid:19786038
  8. 8. Tsakiris M, Hesse MD, Boy C, Haggard P, Fink GR. Neural signatures of body ownership: a sensory network for bodily self-consciousness. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17(10):2235–2244. pmid:17138596
  9. 9. Brizzi G, Sansoni M, Di Lernia D, Frisone F, Tuena C, Riva G. The multisensory mind: a systematic review of multisensory integration processing in Anorexia and Bulimia Nervosa. J Eat Disord. 2023;11(1):204. Published 2023 Nov 16. pmid:37974266
  10. 10. Keizer A, Smeets MA, Dijkerman HC, et al. Tactile body image disturbance in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 2011;190(1):115–120. pmid:21621275
  11. 11. Keizer A, Smeets MA, Dijkerman HC, van Elburg A, Postma A. Aberrant somatosensory perception in Anorexia Nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 2012;200(2–3):530–537. pmid:22648006
  12. 12. Spitoni GF, Serino A, Cotugno A, Mancini F, Antonucci G, Pizzamiglio L. The two dimensions of the body representation in women suffering from Anorexia Nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 2015;230(2):181–188. pmid:26360978
  13. 13. Eshkevari E, Rieger E, Longo MR, Haggard P, Treasure J. Increased plasticity of the bodily self in eating disorders. Psychol Med. 2012;42(4):819–828. pmid:22017964
  14. 14. Guardia D, Carey A, Cottencin O, Thomas P, Luyat M. Disruption of spatial task performance in anorexia nervosa. PLoS One. 2013;8(1):e54928. pmid:23349990
  15. 15. Pollatos O, Kurz AL, Albrecht J, et al. Reduced perception of bodily signals in anorexia nervosa. Eat Behav. 2008;9(4):381–388. pmid:18928900
  16. 16. Zucker NL, Merwin RM, Bulik CM, Moskovich A, Wildes JE, Groh J. Subjective experience of sensation in anorexia nervosa. Behav Res Ther. 2013;51(6):256–265. pmid:23523866
  17. 17. Case LK, Wilson RC, Ramachandran VS. Diminished size-weight illusion in anorexia nervosa: evidence for visuo-proprioceptive integration deficit. Exp Brain Res. 2012;217(1):79–87. pmid:22183754
  18. 18. Gaudio S, Brooks SJ, Riva G. Nonvisual multisensory impairment of body perception in anorexia nervosa: a systematic review of neuropsychological studies. PLoS One. 2014;9(10):e110087. Published 2014 Oct 10. pmid:25303480
  19. 19. Keizer A, Smeets MA, Postma A, van Elburg A, Dijkerman HC. Does the experience of ownership over a rubber hand change body size perception in anorexia nervosa patients?. Neuropsychologia. 2014;62:26–37. pmid:25050852
  20. 20. Chieffi S, Iavarone A, La Marra M, Messina G, Villano I, Ranucci S, et al. Memory for Proprioceptive Targets in Bulimia Nervosa. Afr J Psychiatry. 2015; 18.
  21. 21. Heath DS, Jhinjar N, Hayward DA. Altered social cognition in a community sample of women with disordered eating behaviours: a multi-method approach. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):14683. Published 2021 Jul 19. pmid:34282195
  22. 22. Davies H, Schmidt U, Stahl D, Tchanturia K. Evoked facial emotional expression and emotional experience in people with anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord. 2011;44(6):531–539. pmid:20957704
  23. 23. Oldershaw A, Hambrook D, Stahl D, Tchanturia K, Treasure J, Schmidt U. The socio-emotional processing stream in Anorexia Nervosa. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(3):970–988. pmid:21070808
  24. 24. Russell TA, Schmidt U, Doherty L, Young V, Tchanturia K. Aspects of social cognition in anorexia nervosa: affective and cognitive theory of mind. Psychiatry Res. 2009;168(3):181–185. pmid:19467562
  25. 25. Tchanturia K, Smith E, Weineck F, et al. Exploring autistic traits in anorexia: a clinical study. Mol Autism. 2013;4(1):44. Published 2013 Nov 12. pmid:24220604
  26. 26. Legrand D, Briend F. Anorexia and Bodily Intersubjectivity. Eur Psychol. 2015; 20 (1): 52–61. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000208
  27. 27. Zhang J. Reframing Anorexia Nervosa: A Phenomenological Exploration of the Self-Other Relationship with Husserl’s Intersubjective Theory. Psychopathology. 2024;57(3):229–235. pmid:37751732
  28. 28. Paladino MP, Mazzurega M, Pavani F, Schubert TW. Synchronous multisensory stimulation blurs self-other boundaries. Psychol Sci. 2010;21(9):1202–1207. pmid:20679523
  29. 29. Tajadura-Jiménez A, Grehl S, Tsakiris M. The other in me: interpersonal multisensory stimulation changes the mental representation of the self. PLoS One. 2012;7(7):e40682. pmid:22866177
  30. 30. Tsakiris M. The multisensory basis of the self: From body to identity to others [Formula: see text]. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2017;70(4):597–609. pmid:27100132
  31. 31. Palmer CE, Tsakiris M. Going at the heart of social cognition: is there a role for interoception in self-other distinction?. Curr Opin Psychol. 2018;24:21–26. pmid:29704794
  32. 32. Simantov T, Lombardo M, Baron-Cohen S, Uzefovsky F. Self-Other Distinction. In: Reference Module in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Psychology. Elsevier; 2021.
  33. 33. Brass M, Wiersema JR. Self-Other Distinction and Social Cognition in ASD. In: Volkmar FR, ed. Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2021: 4159–4163.
  34. 34. Cheng-hui Ta N, Shan-shan MA, Chuan-lin ZHU, Yuan Z, Xuan-yi W, De-ming SHU, et al. The self-other distinction in interpersonal interaction: A multi-level exploration. Adv Psychol Sci. 2020; 28 (11): 1890.
  35. 35. Lamm C, Bukowski H, Silani G. From shared to distinct self-other representations in empathy: evidence from neurotypical function and socio-cognitive disorders. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2016;371(1686):20150083. pmid:26644601
  36. 36. Eddy CM. The Transdiagnostic Relevance of Self-Other Distinction to Psychiatry Spans Emotional, Cognitive and Motor Domains. Front Psychiatry. 2022;13:797952. Published 2022 Mar 10. pmid:35360118
  37. 37. Sowden S, Shah P. Self-other control: a candidate mechanism for social cognitive function. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:789. Published 2014 Oct 8. pmid:25339888
  38. 38. Decety J, Sommerville JA. Shared representations between self and other: a social cognitive neuroscience view. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003;7(12):527–533. pmid:14643368
  39. 39. Bird G, Viding E. The self to other model of empathy: providing a new framework for understanding empathy impairments in psychopathy, autism, and alexithymia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;47:520–532. pmid:25454356
  40. 40. Lombardo MV, Chakrabarti B, Bullmore ET, et al. Shared neural circuits for mentalizing about the self and others. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;22(7):1623–1635. pmid:19580380
  41. 41. Steinbeis N, Singer T. Projecting my envy onto you: neurocognitive mechanisms of an offline emotional egocentricity bias. Neuroimage. 2014;102 Pt 2:370–380. pmid:25117603
  42. 42. Hoffmann F, Banzhaf C, Kanske P, Gärtner M, Bermpohl F, Singer T. Empathy in depression: Egocentric and altercentric biases and the role of alexithymia. J Affect Disord. 2016;199:23–29. pmid:27060429
  43. 43. Silani G, Lamm C, Ruff CC, Singer T. Right supramarginal gyrus is crucial to overcome emotional egocentricity bias in social judgments. J Neurosci. 2013;33(39):15466–15476. pmid:24068815
  44. 44. De Meulemeester C, Lowyck B, Panagiotopoulou E, Fotopoulou A, Luyten P. Self-other distinction and borderline personality disorder features: Evidence for egocentric and altercentric bias in a self-other facial morphing task. Personal Disord. 2021;12(4):377–388. pmid:33197197
  45. 45. Keromnes G, Motillon T, Coulon N, et al. Self-other recognition impairments in individuals with schizophrenia: a new experimental paradigm using a double mirror. NPJ Schizophr. 2018;4(1):24. Published 2018 Nov 28. pmid:30487540
  46. 46. Saito N, Yokoyama T, Ohira H. Self-Other Distinction Enhanced Empathic Responses in Individuals with Alexithymia. Sci Rep. 2016;6:35059. Published 2016 Oct 14. pmid:27739448
  47. 47. Kinnaird E, Stewart C, Tchanturia K. Interoception in Anorexia Nervosa: Exploring Associations With Alexithymia and Autistic Traits. Front Psychiatry. 2020;11:64. Published 2020 Feb 21. pmid:32153442
  48. 48. Boltri M, Sapuppo W. Anorexia Nervosa and Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Systematic Review. Psychiatry Res. 2021;306:114271. pmid:34798485
  49. 49. Winnicott DW. Ego Distortion in Terms of True and False Self. In: The Maturational Processes and the Facilitating Environment: Studies in the Theory of Emotional Development. London: Karnac Books; 1960: 140–152.
  50. 50. Chatoor I. Infantile anorexia nervosa: a developmental disorder or separation and individuation. J Am Acad Psychoanal. 1989;17(1):43–64. pmid:2470708
  51. 51. Blatt SJ, Blass RB. Attachment and separateness. A dialectic model of the products and processes of development throughout the life cycle. Psychoanal Study Child. 1990;45:107–127. pmid:2251302
  52. 52. Tasca GA, Balfour L. Attachment and eating disorders: a review of current research. Int J Eat Disord. 2014;47(7):710–717. pmid:24862477
  53. 53. Kerr-Gaffney J, Harrison A, Tchanturia K. Cognitive and Affective Empathy in Eating Disorders: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Front Psychiatry. 2019;10:102. Published 2019 Mar 4. pmid:30886590
  54. 54. Berthoz A, Thirioux B. A Spatial and Perspective Change Theory of the Difference Between Sympathy and Empathy. In: Reference Module in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Psychology. Elsevier; 2010.
  55. 55. Guardia D, Cottencin O, Thomas P, Dodin V, Luyat M. Spatial orientation constancy is impaired in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 2012;195(1–2):56–59. pmid:21872340
  56. 56. Nico D, Daprati E. The egocentric reference for visual exploration and orientation. Brain Cogn. 2009;69(2):227–235. pmid:18752882
  57. 57. Frith U, de Vignemont F. Egocentrism, allocentrism, and Asperger syndrome. Conscious Cogn. 2005;14(4):719–738. pmid:15996486
  58. 58. Decety J, Lamm C. The role of the right temporoparietal junction in social interaction: how low-level computational processes contribute to meta-cognition. Neuroscientist. 2007;13(6):580–593. pmid:17911216
  59. 59. Sachdev P, Mondraty N, Wen W, Gulliford K. Brains of anorexia nervosa patients process self-images differently from non-self-images: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(8):2161–2168. pmid:18406432
  60. 60. Vocks S, Busch M, Grönemeyer D, Schulte D, Herpertz S, Suchan B. Neural correlates of viewing photographs of one’s own body and another woman’s body in anorexia and bulimia nervosa: an fMRI study. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2010;35(3):163–176. pmid:20420767
  61. 61. Devue C, Collette F, Balteau E, et al. Here I am: the cortical correlates of visual self-recognition. Brain Res. 2007;1143:169–182. pmid:17306235
  62. 62. Boehme R, Olausson H. Differentiating self-touch from social touch. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2022;43:27–33.
  63. 63. Cascio CJ, Moore D, McGlone F. Social touch and human development. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2019;35:5–11. pmid:29731417
  64. 64. Castiello U, Becchio C, Zoia S, et al. Wired to be social: the ontogeny of human interaction. PLoS One. 2010;5(10):e13199. Published 2010 Oct 7. pmid:20949058
  65. 65. Ciaunica A, Seth A, Limanowski J, Hesp C, Friston KJ. I overthink-Therefore I am not: An active inference account of altered sense of self and agency in depersonalisation disorder. Conscious Cogn. 2022;101:103320. pmid:35490544
  66. 66. Della Longa L, Filippetti ML, Dragovic D, Farroni T. Synchrony of Caresses: Does Affective Touch Help Infants to Detect Body-Related Visual-Tactile Synchrony?. Front Psychol. 2020;10:2944. Published 2020 Jan 9. pmid:31998194
  67. 67. McGlone F, Wessberg J, Olausson H. Discriminative and affective touch: sensing and feeling. Neuron. 2014;82(4):737–755. pmid:24853935
  68. 68. Morrison I, Löken LS, Olausson H. The skin as a social organ. Exp Brain Res. 2010;204(3):305–314. pmid:19771420
  69. 69. Keizer A, Heijman JO, Dijkerman HC. Do transdiagnostic factors influence affective touch perception in psychiatric populations? Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2022; 43:125–130.
  70. 70. Crucianelli L, Cardi V, Treasure J, Jenkinson PM, Fotopoulou A. The perception of affective touch in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 2016;239:72–78. pmid:27137964
  71. 71. Crucianelli L, Demartini B, Goeta D, et al. The Anticipation and Perception of Affective Touch in Women with and Recovered from Anorexia Nervosa. Neuroscience. 2021;464:143–155. pmid:32937191
  72. 72. Davidovic M, Karjalainen L, Starck G, Wentz E, Björnsdotter M, Olausson H. Abnormal brain processing of gentle touch in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2018;281:53–60. pmid:30248526
  73. 73. Bischoff-Grethe A, Wierenga CE, Berner LA, et al. Neural hypersensitivity to pleasant touch in women remitted from anorexia nervosa. Transl Psychiatry. 2018;8(1):161. Published 2018 Aug 16. pmid:30115929
  74. 74. Frost-Karlsson M, Capusan AJ, Perini I, et al. Neural processing of self-touch and other-touch in anorexia nervosa and autism spectrum condition. Neuroimage Clin. 2022;36:103264. pmid:36451367
  75. 75. Heinisch C, Dinse HR, Tegenthoff M, Juckel G, Brüne M. An rTMS study into self-face recognition using video-morphing technique. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2011;6(4):442–449. pmid:20587597
  76. 76. Uddin LQ, Molnar-Szakacs I, Zaidel E, Iacoboni M. rTMS to the right inferior parietal lobule disrupts self-other discrimination. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2006;1(1):65–71. pmid:17387382
  77. 77. Hirot F, Lesage M, Pedron L, et al. Impaired processing of self-face recognition in anorexia nervosa. Eat Weight Disord. 2016;21(1):31–40. pmid:26420298
  78. 78. Arzy S, Overney LS, Landis T, Blanke O. (2006). Neural Mechanisms of Embodiment: Asomatognosia Due to Premotor Cortex Damage. Arch Neurol. 63(7):1022–1025. pmid:16831974
  79. 79. Legrand D. The bodily self: The sensori-motor roots of pre-reflective self-consciousness. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences. 2006; 5 (1).
  80. 80. Cascino G, Castellini G, Stanghellini G, et al. The Role of the Embodiment Disturbance in the Anorexia Nervosa Psychopathology: A Network Analysis Study. Brain Sci. 2019;9(10):276. Published 2019 Oct 15. pmid:31619011
  81. 81. Fuchs T. The disappearing body: anorexia as a conflict of embodiment. Eat Weight Disord. 2022;27(1):109–117. pmid:33666885
  82. 82. Guardia D, Conversy L, Jardri R, et al. Imagining one’s own and someone else’s body actions: dissociation in anorexia nervosa. PLoS One. 2012;7(8):e43241. pmid:22937025
  83. 83. Thirioux B, Wehrmann M, Langbour N, Jaafari N, Berthoz A. Identifying Oneself with the Face of Someone Else Impairs the Egocentered Visuo-spatial Mechanisms: A New Double Mirror Paradigm to Study Self-other Distinction and Interaction. Front Psychol. 2016;7:1283. Published 2016 Aug 25. pmid:27610095
  84. 84. Lavenne-Collot N, Tersiguel M, Dissaux N, et al. Self/other distinction in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) assessed with a double mirror paradigm. PLoS One. 2023;18(3):e0275018. Published 2023 Mar 16. pmid:36928493
  85. 85. Amsterdam B. Mirror self-image reactions before age two. Dev Psychobiol. 1972;5(4):297–305. pmid:4679817
  86. 86. Keenan JP, Gallup GG, Falk D. The Face in the Mirror: The Search for the Origins of Consciousness. HarperCollins Publishers; 2003: xxiii, 278.
  87. 87. Rochat P, Striano T. Who’s in the mirror? Self-other discrimination in specular images by four- and nine-month-old infants. Child Dev. 2002;73(1):35–46. pmid:14717242
  88. 88. Zazzo R. Mirror Image and Anti-Mirror Image: Experiences on the Formation of Self-Image. Enfance. 1977; 30 (2–4): 223–230.
  89. 89. Mancini M, Esposito CM. Lived body and the Other’s gaze: a phenomenological perspective on feeding and eating disorders. Eat Weight Disord. 2021;26(8):2523–2529. pmid:33544360
  90. 90. Sartre JP. Being and Nothingness: An Essay in Phenomenological Ontology. 1st ed. Translated by Richmond S. New York, NY: Routledge; 2018. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429434013
  91. 91. Sproull L, Subramani M, Kiesler S, Walker JH, Waters K. When the Interface Is a Face. Hum Comput Interact. 1996; 11 (2): 97–124. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327051hci1102_1
  92. 92. Anderson AJ, Vingrys AJ. Small samples: does size matter?. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2001;42(7):1411–1413. pmid:11381039
  93. 93. Indrayan A, Mishra A. The importance of small samples in medical research. J Postgrad Med. 2021;67(4):219–223. pmid:34845889
  94. 94. American Psychiatric Association (APA). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®). 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.
  95. 95. World Health Organization (WHO). The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva, Switzerland: WHO; 1992.
  96. 96. Assaiante C, Barlaam F, Cignetti F, Vaugoyeau M. Body schema building during childhood and adolescence: a neurosensory approach. Neurophysiol Clin. 2014;44(1):3–12. pmid:24502900
  97. 97. Keromnes G, Chokron S, Celume MP, et al. Exploring Self-Consciousness From Self- and Other-Image Recognition in the Mirror: Concepts and Evaluation. Front Psychol. 2019;10:719. Published 2019 May 7. pmid:31133909
  98. 98. Bruch H. Anorexia Nervosa: therapy and theory. Am J Psychiatry. 1982;139(12):1531–1538. pmid:6816075
  99. 99. Amianto F, Northoff G, Abbate Daga G, Fassino S, Tasca GA. Is Anorexia Nervosa a Disorder of the Self? A Psychological Approach. Front Psychol. 2016;7:849. Published 2016 Jun 14. pmid:27378967
  100. 100. Williams K, King J, Fox JR. Sense of self and anorexia nervosa: A grounded theory. Psychol Psychother. 2016;89(2):211–228. pmid:26179295
  101. 101. Tsakiris M. Looking for myself: current multisensory input alters self-face recognition. PLoS One. 2008;3(12):e4040. pmid:19107208
  102. 102. Courage ML, Edison SC, Howe ML, Howe M. Variability in the Early Development of Visual Self-Recognition. Infant Behav Dev. 2004; 27 (4): 509–532.
  103. 103. Butler DL, Mattingley JB, Cunnington R, Suddendorf T. Mirror, mirror on the wall, how does my brain recognize my image at all?. PLoS One. 2012;7(2):e31452. pmid:22359592
  104. 104. Breen N, Caine D, Coltheart M. Mirrored-self misidentification: two cases of focal onset dementia. Neurocase. 2001;7(3):239–254. pmid:11459919
  105. 105. Botvinick M, Cohen J. Rubber hands ’feel’ touch that eyes see. Nature. 1998;391(6669):756. pmid:9486643
  106. 106. Sforza A, Bufalari I, Haggard P, Aglioti SM. My face in yours: Visuo-tactile facial stimulation influences sense of identity. Soc Neurosci. 2010;5(2):148–162. pmid:19813138
  107. 107. Apps MA, Tajadura-Jiménez A, Sereno M, Blanke O, Tsakiris M. Plasticity in unimodal and multimodal brain areas reflects multisensory changes in self-face identification. Cereb Cortex. 2015;25(1):46–55. pmid:23964067
  108. 108. Kilteni K, Maselli A, Kording KP, Slater M. Over my fake body: body ownership illusions for studying the multisensory basis of own-body perception. Front Hum Neurosci. 2015;9:141. Published 2015 Mar 24. pmid:25852524
  109. 109. Zopf R, Contini E, Fowler C, Mondraty N, Williams MA. Body distortions in Anorexia Nervosa: Evidence for changed processing of multisensory bodily signals. Psychiatry Res. 2016;245:473–481. pmid:27639162
  110. 110. Keizer A, van Elburg A, Helms R, Dijkerman HC. A Virtual Reality Full Body Illusion Improves Body Image Disturbance in Anorexia Nervosa. PLoS One. 2016;11(10):e0163921. Published 2016 Oct 6. pmid:27711234
  111. 111. Provenzano L, Porciello G, Ciccarone S, et al. Characterizing Body Image Distortion and Bodily Self-Plasticity in Anorexia Nervosa via Visuo-Tactile Stimulation in Virtual Reality. J Clin Med. 2019;9(1):98. Published 2019 Dec 30. pmid:31906009
  112. 112. Porciello G, Bufalari I, Minio-Paluello I, Di Pace E, Aglioti SM. The ’Enfacement’ illusion: A window on the plasticity of the self. Cortex. 2018;104:261–275. pmid:29478669
  113. 113. Demartini B, Nisticò V, Tedesco R, et al. Visual perception and dissociation during Mirror Gazing Test in patients with anorexia nervosa: a preliminary study. Eat Weight Disord. 2021;26(5):1541–1551. pmid:32757140
  114. 114. Bastiaansen JA, Thioux M, Keysers C. Evidence for mirror systems in emotions. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2009;364(1528):2391–2404. pmid:19620110
  115. 115. Freud S. Civilizationand Its Discontents. Toronto, ON: Broadview Press; 1930
  116. 116. Feinberg TE, Keenan JP. Where in the brain is the self?. Conscious Cogn. 2005;14(4):661–678. pmid:16325140
  117. 117. McIntosh VV, Bulik CM, McKenzie JM, Luty SE, Jordan J. Interpersonal psychotherapy for anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord. 2000;27(2):125–139. pmid:10657886
  118. 118. Quagliata E. Some Reflections on the Processes of Projection and Introjection in Eating Disorders. In: Exploring Eating Disorders in Adolescents. Routledge; 2004
  119. 119. Steinbeis N. The role of self-other distinction in understanding others’ mental and emotional states: neurocognitive mechanisms in children and adults [published correction appears in Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2016 Feb 19;371(1688):20150547.]. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2016;371(1686):20150074. pmid:26644593
  120. 120. Friston K, FitzGerald T, Rigoli F, Schwartenbeck P, Pezzulo G. Active Inference: A Process Theory. Neural Comput. 2017;29(1):1–49. pmid:27870614
  121. 121. Pezzulo G, Rigoli F, Friston K. Active Inference, homeostatic regulation and adaptive behavioural control. Prog Neurobiol. 2015;134:17–35. pmid:26365173
  122. 122. Gadsby S, Hohwy J. Why Use Predictive Processing to Explain Psychopathology? The Case of Anorexia Nervosa. In: Mendonca D, Curado M, Gouveia S, eds. The Philosophy and Science of Predictive Processing. London, UK: Bloomsbury Academic; 2021: 209–226.
  123. 123. Kilteni K, Andersson BJ, Houborg C, Ehrsson HH. Motor imagery involves predicting the sensory consequences of the imagined movement. Nat Commun. 2018;9(1):1617. Published 2018 Apr 24. pmid:29691389
  124. 124. Boehme R, Hauser S, Gerling GJ, Heilig M, Olausson H. Distinction of self-produced touch and social touch at cortical and spinal cord levels. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2019;116(6):2290–2299. pmid:30670645
  125. 125. Blakemore SJ, Wolpert DM, Frith CD. Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation. Nat Neurosci. 1998;1(7):635–640. pmid:10196573
  126. 126. Riva G, Gaudio S. Locked to a wrong body: Eating disorders as the outcome of a primary disturbance in multisensory body integration. Conscious Cogn. 2018;59:57–59. pmid:28865673
  127. 127. Bullinger A. Le Développement Sensori-Moteur de l’Enfant et Ses Avatars, Un Parcours de Recherche Erès. Ramonville-St-Agne, France: Éditions Érès; 2004. Pierre Delion.
  128. 128. Kloeckner A, Jutard C, Bullinger A, Nicoulaud L, Tordjman S, Cohen D. Intérêt de l’abord sensorimoteur dans les pathologies autistiques sévères I: Introduction aux travaux d’André Bullinger. Neuropsychiatr Enfance Adolesc. 2009; 57 (2): 154–159. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurenf.2008.12.002
  129. 129. Rahme IP, Oliveira EP de. The Effect of Back Support on Attention in Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Pilot Study Testing André Bullinger’s Model. Enfance. 2015; 4 (4): 515–524.
  130. 130. Grunwald M, Weiss T. Inducing sensory stimulation in treatment of anorexia nervosa. QJM. 2005;98(5):379–380. pmid:15833769
  131. 131. Kolnes LJ. Embodying the body in anorexia nervosa—a physiotherapeutic approach. J Bodyw Mov Ther. 2012;16(3):281–288. pmid:22703737
  132. 132. Winberg A, Persson I, Thorssell M. Kroppsuppfattning hos patienter med anorexia och bulimia nervosa-en studie av några sjukgymnastiska undersøkningsmetoder [Body perception among patients with anorexia and bulimia nervosa-a study of some physiotherapeutic examination methods]. Nordisk Fysioterapi. 1997; 1: 18–28.
  133. 133. Cimolin V, Galli M, Vismara L, et al. Gait analysis in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. J Appl Biomater Funct Mater. 2013;11(2):e122–e128. Published 2013 Sep 13. pmid:23728541
  134. 134. Naess CR, Kolnes LJ. A preliminary exploration of experiences of integrating the body in the self in two women with anorexia nervosa in view of phenomenological conceptualisations. J Eat Disord. 2022;10(1):153. Published 2022 Nov 1. pmid:36320024
  135. 135. Nico D, Daprati E, Nighoghossian N, Carrier E, Duhamel JR, Sirigu A. The role of the right parietal lobe in anorexia nervosa. Psychol Med. 2010;40(9):1531–1539. pmid:19917144
  136. 136. Saint-Georges C, Cassel RS, Cohen D, Chetouani M, Laznik MC, Maestro S, et al. What studies of family home movies can teach us about autistic infants: A literature review. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2010; 4 (3): 355–366.
  137. 137. Quesque F, Brass M. The Role of the Temporoparietal Junction in Self-Other Distinction. Brain Topogr. 2019;32(6):943–955. pmid:31676934
  138. 138. Miles S, Gnatt I, Phillipou A, Nedeljkovic M. Cognitive flexibility in acute anorexia nervosa and after recovery: A systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2020;81:101905. pmid:32891022
  139. 139. Dahlenburg SC, Gleaves DH, Hutchinson AD. Anorexia nervosa and perfectionism: A meta-analysis. Int J Eat Disord. 2019;52(3):219–229. pmid:30632629