Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Identifying the impact of social influences in health-related discrete choice experiments

  • Esther W. de Bekker-Grob ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    debekker-grob@eshpm.eur.nl

    Affiliations Erasmus Choice Modelling Centre, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, The Netherlands, Erasmus School of Health Policy & Management, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, The Netherlands

  • Kirsten Howard,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Public Health, Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia

  • Joffre Swait

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Erasmus Choice Modelling Centre, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, The Netherlands, Erasmus School of Health Policy & Management, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, The Netherlands

Abstract

Several disciplines, among them health, sociology, and economics, provide strong evidence that social context is important to individual choices. It is therefore surprising that relatively little research has been focused on integrating the effect of social influence into choice models, especially given the importance of such choices in healthcare. This study developed and empirically tested a choice model that accounts for social network influences in a discrete choice experiment (DCE). We focused on maternal choices for childhood vaccination in Australia, and used an econometric choice model that explicitly 1) incorporated vaccine schedule characteristics, benefits and costs, and 2) represented up to ten different identifiable key influencer types (e.g., partner, parents, friends, healthcare professionals, inter alia), allowing for the attribution of directional importance of each influencer on the gravid woman’s decision to adhere to or reject childhood vaccination. Pregnant women (N = 604) aged 18 years and older recruited from an online panel completed a survey, including a DCE and questions about key influencers. A two-class ordered latent class model was conducted to analyse the DCE data, which assumes that the underlying latent driver (in our case the WHO vaccine hesitancy scale) is ordered, to give a practical interpretation of the meaning of the classes. When the choice model considered both childhood vaccination attributes and key influencers, a very high model fit was reached. The impact of key influencers on maternal choice for childhood vaccination was massive compared to the impact of childhood vaccination attributes. The marginal impact differed between key influencers. Our DCE study showed that the maternal decision for childhood vaccination was essentially almost completely socially driven, suggesting that the potential impact of social network influences can and should be considered in health-related DCEs, particular those where there are likely to be strong underlying social norms dictating decision maker behaviour.

1. Introduction

Choice modelling is mainstream in marketing, transport and environmental economics, where it is used–among other applications–for transport policy development, urban planning systems, marketing product pricing, and resource management decision-making [1, 2]. Choice modelling is also increasingly used in health economics. Since its introduction in the early nineties, the stated choice technique ‘Discrete Choice Experiment (DCE)’, originating from mathematical psychology [3], has become increasingly popular [47]. It has been used–among other applications–for informing resource planning and predicting uptake where there is no information or trial data [47]. Although choice modelling via DCEs has a huge potential to support decision-making in healthcare, some of the usual assumptions made in their application limit their usefulness. Traditionally, almost universally in health, transport, marketing and environment applications of choice models assume that choice processes are independent of the influence of other people than the decision-maker (e.g., the patient, the physician, health policy maker) in question (or rather, they simply do not explore understanding of preferences). However, and most especially in healthcare, choices are not made in a social vacuum.

It is surprising that relatively little research has been focused on integrating the effect of social influence into choice models [8], especially given the importance of such choices in healthcare [9, 10]. This statement is made with the recognition that several disciplines, among them health, sociology, and economics, provide strong evidence that social context is important to individual choices (see, e.g., the health literature concerning MMR vaccine to protect against measles, mumps, and rubella [1113]). At most, outside the healthcare area, there is existence of econometric corrections that might arise from supposed social influences [14], and approaches to measure and model joint choices (e.g. household decisions) or effects of attitudes of peers on the decision-making process of the individual [8, 15].

As most DCE surveys in healthcare are not designed to isolate social influences arising from the decision-makers’ context, the aim of this study was to develop and empirically test a choice model that accounts for social network influences in a health-related DCE. Hence, in our study, we seek to isolate this one aspect of context, which we thought a priori should have a substantial impact on decision-makers: influences of social network. That is, careful thought suggested to us that this particular source is a potentially very important type of influence establishing people’s behaviour. To test this hypothesis, the choice model we developed—as preferences are one way to understand influences of social network on decision-makers—was expanding the decision model for this particular type of influence.

We focused on maternal choices for childhood vaccination in Australia, and used an econometric choice model that explicitly 1) incorporated vaccine schedule characteristics, benefits and costs (i.e. vaccine schedule attributes), and 2) represented up to ten different identifiable key influencer types (including partner, parents, friends, healthcare professionals), allowing for the attribution of importance (and direction) of each influencer on the gravid woman’s decision to adhere to or reject childhood vaccination. As the DCE method is a richer method compared to the direct survey question–that is, it is able to jointly take childhood vaccination schedule attributes and social influences into account, while also considering the respondent’s trade-offs between the variables, between key influencers and between variables and key influencers–we hypothesise that the DCE outcomes are more informative and therefore more useful for policy decision-making (e.g., identification and targeting of particular messages or conveyers of messages to particular respondent types).

2. Methods

For several reasons we focused on childhood vaccination decisions (more specifically, vaccinations over the first 12 months) in Australia: (a) subjects’ choices for childhood vaccination are likely highly sensitive to social influences due to the strong underlying social norm (including current Australian policy response [16] to vaccine hesitancy [17]), which is vital to empirically test a choice model that accounts for social network influences; (b) declines in childhood vaccination rates in Australia (and worldwide) have received media coverage in the last few years, contributing to the relevance of the study; and (c) understanding broader influences on vaccination decisions may help determine targeting of appropriate education, behaviour change strategies and/or policy. A labelled DCE survey [18], that was suitable to explain real-life choices and able to deal with social influences, was conducted. Approval for the study was obtained from the Human Research Ethics Committee, University of South-Australia (PG082203).

2.1. Literature review and attribute selection

Childhood vaccination decisions can be conceptualised as occurring along a continuum, with full adherence to recommended vaccination schedules at one end and no vaccination at the other. That is, childhood vaccination is a preventive intervention, so individuals are not obliged to get vaccinated against diseases. Therefore, parents may choose to delay or split scheduled vaccinations, or to partly vaccinate. This so-called vaccine hesitancy [17] is however not without risk. It has significant effects on population disease risk [19, 20]. To identify vaccine schedule attributes (including attribute levels) that may impact maternal choices to opt for 1) the recommended vaccination schedule, 2) a delay or split scheduled vaccination, or 3) no vaccination at all, we used a literature search among stated preference and qualitative studies in childhood vaccination [2134], and the Australian National Immunisation Program (NIP) Schedule for young children as at August 2015. Thereafter, based on the literature review and the Australian Immunisation Schedule, the most relevant childhood vaccination schedule attributes and their consequences were included in the labelled DCE. The attributes and their alternative specific levels are presented in Table 1.

thumbnail
Table 1. Attributes and alternative specific levels for childhood vaccination in Australia.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276141.t001

2.2. Discrete choice experiment (DCE)

The labelled DCE design contained three alternatives for each choice task (i.e., a recommended vaccination schedule profile (fixed), a certain delay or split scheduled vaccination profile (variation), and a ‘no vaccination’ profile (fixed)). We deliberately chose to mirror the real choices that parents would be making with respect to vaccination–a fixed schedule, as per recommended (A); a variable schedule with possible delays/splits (B), and a fixed no vaccination schedule (C)–with only schedule B varying in each question. In each choice task, respondents were asked to opt for the alternative that appealed most to them. The combination of eight attributes and their corresponding levels resulted into many potential delays/splits scheduled childhood vaccination profiles. To create a much smaller subset of childhood vaccination scenarios (i.e. choice tasks) with little loss of information or estimation precision, a Bayesian D-efficient DCE design was used [35]. We generated a DCE design consisting of 600 choice tasks blocked into 50 sub-designs using NGene software [36]. Each respondent was randomly assigned to a sub-design containing 12 discrete choice tasks each.

To reduce respondent burden further and to be as clear as possible, the attribute levels of ‘the delay or split scheduled vaccination’ profile were presented as a direct comparison with the ‘fixed recommended vaccination schedule’ profile. See S1 Appendix for more details. The information we presented about the attributes and their levels, was consistent with the Australian NIP Schedule as at August 2015, existing and newly considered government policies with respect to consequences for delayed or no vaccination and parental information brochures provided by the NIP and general practitioners to minimise information asymmetry between the hypothetical situations representing the actual decision and the actual decision. Hence, attribute wording was deliberately made consistent with the words and descriptions that were used in the NIP Schedule and are therefore the words that parents would be presented with when making the actual decisions about vaccination. Informal piloting of these attribute descriptions did not indicate any difficulties with understanding and interpretation of the wording.

2.3. Survey and sample

Besides the 12 DCE choice tasks (for the estimation of the decision model) and questions regarding respondents’ characteristics, the survey included questions where respondents were asked to consider and assess statements about vaccines and vaccinations using validated questions from Zingg et al. [37] and Larson et al. [38]. That is, to be able to detect substantial correlations between knowledge and willingness to vaccinate, Zingg et al. [37] developed a one-dimensional knowledge scale about vaccination with good psychometric properties. To help diagnose and address vaccine hesitancy, Larson et al. [38] developed a matrix that mapped the key factors influencing the decision to accept, delay or reject some or all vaccines under three categories: contextual, individual and group, and vaccine-specific. Additionally, respondents were asked to assess their susceptibility to social influences with respect to health behaviours using validated questions from Holt et al. [39]. That is, to answer some of the questions about the role of social networks and social influence processes on health behaviours and outcomes, Holt et al. [39] developed and validated an instrument to assess the perceived role of others in the health behaviour decisions of individuals. Respondents were also asked about different people (‘key influencers’ or KIs) in their life and the role KIs might play in their decision to vaccinate their child. This included questions such as ‘Would this person(s) be a possible source of health information?’; ‘How long have you known this person?’; ‘How important is this person’s view on your decision to vaccinate your child?’; and ‘For <key influencer type X>, please tell us how likely you believe they would be to recommend each vaccination option by allocating 100 points across the three options for this person, with more points meaning that they are more likely to recommend that option’. The survey also included questions related to childhood vaccination knowledge, the WHO vaccine hesitancy scale (which mapped key factors influencing the decision to accept, delay or reject some or all vaccines under three categories: contextual, individual and group, and vaccine specific),38 and attitudinal questions. The DCE survey is available from the authors upon request.

An online sample of 604 pregnant women from the Australian general population, nationally representative in terms of age, education, and geographic region was recruited via Survey Sampling International (currently known as Dynata) and Bounty (an Australian parent specific panel; www.bountyparents.com.au/). Calculation of optimal sample sizes for a DCE is complicated as it depends on the true values of the unknown parameters in the assumed discrete choice model [40]. Taking the aim of our study into account, we opted to be parsimonious and focus on identifying the relative role of social influences of maternal choices (hence, we were less interested in how accurate the model could be with respect to uptake estimation of a certain childhood vaccination program − i.e., we were not setting out our research to predict vaccine uptake as accurately as possible). Accordingly, our sample size was based on affordability, which suggested using a convenience sample with a sample size that was sufficient for study purposes. See S2 Appendix for more details about the sample size calculation. The respondents were incentivized according to the online panel companies’ incentive scheme. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants for inclusion in the study. We utilized all respondents that completed (i.e., did not abandon) the survey before it was closed off at the end of the data collection period (see S3 Appendix for more details).

2.4. DCE analysis

Several models exist to analyse discrete choice data [7, 41, 42]. Each choice model has its set of features, which should fit the intentions of the research and match the respondents’ choice behaviour. Given our interest in accounting for social network influences, while also taking our sample size and the risk of overfitting into account, led to the decision to employ (after exploring a traditional multinomial model logit (MNL) model first) a panel ordered latent class model (OLCM) using custom-developed software in Fortran 77. A latent class model can be used to identify the existence and the number of segments or classes, M, in the population (i.e., identifying different utility (preference) functions across unobserved subgroups). Class membership is latent in that each respondent belongs to each class up to a modelled probability and not deterministically assigned by the analyst a priori. It is noteworthy that the ordered LCM model is flexible in that the probability that sampled respondents belong to a particular class can be linked to the underlying ordinal latent driver (in our case the WHO vaccine hesitancy scale), hence allowing for some understanding as to the make-up of the various class segments (i.e., a neat way to classify respondents). (See more details in the S4 Appendix.) This additional information can be really useful for policy makers to help them to develop appropriate and tailored education, behaviour change strategies and/or policy. We used a panel LCM, as it accounts for the panel nature of the data since each respondent completed 12 discrete choice tasks. We systematically tested standard (i.e. nominal) and ordered LCMs with different classes, and several different specifications for the utility function (e.g., categorical or numerical attribute levels, two-way interactions between attributes, several attribute transformations). To account for social network influences and modelling our DCE data by assuming a utility maximisation decision process, we used the conditional choice model formulated by Swait & Marley [43] extended for multiple influencers (see S4 Appendix for more details).

The final model was based on parsimoniousness and Bayesian Information Criterion outcomes and resulted in a two-class ordered latent class (2-OLCM) model, which assumed that the underlying classes are ordered on a single latent dimension (in our case, the WHO vaccine hesitance scale) [44]. Ordered latent class models are not new to the econometric literature (e.g., Gopinath and Ben-Akiva [45]; Swait and Sweeney [46]; Swait and Adamowicz [47]), but its frequency of use is much lower than its categorical counterpart (e.g., among many others, Huls et al. [48]). We detail the full model in the S4 Appendix, to which we direct the reader, whereas in S5 Appendix more details about the DCE analysis can be found.

When the choice model considered both childhood vaccination attributes and key influencers, a very high model fit was reached. The utilities of the different vaccination schedules are specified on the basis of two types of factors: (1) schedule characteristics (see Table 1) and (2) key influencer impacts. To interpret the schedule characteristics coefficients, a positive effect indicates that schedule characteristic makes vaccination more likely, while a negative effect indicates vaccination becomes less likely with increases.

The key influencers are represented with terms such as ‘ln(Partner), ln(Mother), …’, which describe the potential effect on the respondent’s choice of that actor through the respondent’s perception of what would be the actor’s recommended behaviour. These key influencer effects are further deemed to be potentially moderated by two respondent characteristics: their level of knowledge about vaccination (K) and their susceptibility to social influences (S). These moderating characteristics were measured using established scales (for K, see Zingg and Siegrist [37]; for S, see Holt et al. [39]).

The sign of the key influencer coefficients (including moderation interactions) reflects whether the key influencer has a positive or negative effect on the utility for childhood vaccination schedule. The magnitude of the class assignment threshold τ indicates the cut-off value of the WHO vaccine hesitancy scale separating two (consecutive) classes. That is, the probability to belong to a specific class is determined by the WHO vaccine hesitancy scale value.

2.5. Impact of social influences on childhood vaccination decision

Using the estimated coefficients from the ordered 2-class model, the average marginal impact of each key influencer (KI) on the utility of a vaccination schedule was determined. By assumption/construction of the choice models, the estimated effects for each KI are interpreted as orthogonal (or independent) of the others; this assumption is held in common with all regression-type statistical models. As indicated earlier, our model specifications allowed for these KI impacts to be moderated by two respondent characteristics, K (Knowledge about vaccination) and S (Susceptibility to social influences). These interaction terms capture potential heterogeneity across respondents (mothers) in terms of the impact of KIs.

In addition, the changes in utility due to the level of the key influencers’ support for the recommended schedule were calculated via one-way sensitivity analysis. Essentially, we map out the conditional main effect (i.e., interaction terms set to nil) of each KI on the utility of a vaccination schedule. And finally, we characterized the aggregate effect of the value of social level consensus for the recommended childhood vaccination schedule by building the cumulative key influencer group effect one by one (i.e. repeatedly adding one KI at a time), starting with the partner. Thus, these statistics concerning the impact of key influencers will give us a two-fold perspective on social influences: first, at the individual KI level, controlling for all other KIs; second, at the aggregate/group level, by demonstrating how influential multiple KIs can be.

3. Results

3.1. Respondents

In total 604 pregnant women aged 18 years and older participated in the online panel survey concerning childhood vaccination decisions. The Dynata panel provided 404 respondents, and another 200 were recruited from the Bounty Panel. These respondents had a mean age of 31 years (SD = 4.8) and were on average 26 weeks pregnant (SD = 9.0) (Table 2). About 53% of the sample had at least a bachelor’s degree. Approximately 96% of the respondents reported that they were vaccinated as a child, and 37% of the respondents mentioned that they had one or more children at the time of the survey. The mean score on the vaccine hesitancy scale, according to our scale calculation method which generated sample values ranging from -20 (no hesitancy) to +15 (high hesitancy), was -10.3 (SD = 7.1), with 6.1% having a score higher than the threshold score suggesting behaviour consistent with high vaccine hesitancy (i.e., membership in class 1—we return to this topic shortly). Almost 97%, 91% and 87% of the sample had a partner, a friend with children, and a usual GP respectively (Table 3). Persons that were mainly considered as sources for health information were the usual GP followed by the usual other health care professional, the usual pharmacist, and the friend with children (97.0, 94.7, 87.1 and 70.4%, respectively). It is clear from Table 3 that respondents have a wide range of perceptions concerning who is a health information source for themselves, which is suggestive that these potential sources could be differentially impactful on their choices.

3.2. DCE results

Seventy-eight percent of the respondents only opted for the recommended schedule, whereas less than three percent of the respondents only opted for the delayed schedule or only opted-out for vaccination. Essentially, 117 out of 604 respondents (19.4%) could be persuaded to trade between the alternatives. When the LCM-2 model considered both childhood vaccination attributes and key influencers, a very high model fit was reached (pseudo-R2 = 0.825) (Table 4). (Note that a 3-class model was also estimated, but a comparison of BIC measures ultimately led to the selection of the 2-class model.) The average class probabilities within the sampled population were 10.9% and 89.1% for latent classes 1 and 2, respectively. The probability to belong to a specific class depended strongly on the WHO vaccine hesitancy scale value. That is, membership in the classes is determined by responses to the 10 measurement items shown in Table 4 (section labelled Classification Scoring Function: WHO_SAGE Vaccine Hesitancy Scale), which are aggregated/summed together using the signed unit weights (±1’s) also shown in Table 4. Making these weights explicit serves two functions: 1) it makes explicit that some items have positive or negative valence; and 2) shows that these weights were not estimated from the data, but defined so as to allow direct and easy subsequent use of the classification model in any advisory tools that might be developed from our results. The items themselves were coded according to the coding scheme also included in the table under the heading “Responses on Likert scale (code for scoring function)”. The resulting WHO vaccine hesitancy score (= sum(weights * item responses)) ranges from -20 to +20, though empirically ranged from -20 to +15 in the sample (see Table 3). A user can apply the scoring mechanism above to determine class membership for a randomly selected mother in the target Australian sub-population, and thence have preference estimates for that mother without having to conduct a DCE. To be more precise, if the respondent had a vaccine hesitancy score of [-20, 1.1304], she belonged to class #2, but if she had a vaccine hesitancy score of [1.1304, +20] she belonged to class #1 (see Table 4 and Fig 1). A threshold τ-value of 1.1304 on the WHO vaccine hesitancy was identified by the model as the cut-off value between class #1 and class #2.

thumbnail
Table 4. DCE results based on an aggregate MNL and a 2-class ordered latent class model.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276141.t004

The estimated coefficients for each latent class showed that the impact of key influencers on the maternal choice for childhood vaccination was massive (in magnitude and significance) compared to the impact of childhood vaccination attributes (Table 4). To exemplify, in Table 4, ‘Friends with children’ has a strong impact on the respondent such that if the respondent believes the friend with children would recommend vaccination, she would be more likely to vaccinate; however, this influencer’s impact would be largely mitigated if the respondent is deemed more knowledgeable about vaccination, but the impact would be increased if the respondent is deemed more susceptible to social influences. This result, coupled with the extremely high fit of the 2-class model (as noted above, R2 = 0.825), suggests that for these respondents and this choice context, the key influencers were so important in making their choices in the DCE, that the decision for childhood vaccination can be safely interpreted as being purely socially driven.

3.3. Impact of social influences on childhood vaccination decision

The marginal impact on the respondents’ utility of a childhood vaccination schedule differed between key influencers. Based on the DCE coefficients, the partner of the gravid woman had by far the largest impact on the maternal choice for childhood vaccination, followed by the gravid woman’s mother, and thirdly, friends with children (Fig 2). This result was partly confirmed by the direct survey question regarding how important the view of a certain key influencer was (convergent validity), where the partner of the gravid woman was also mentioned to be the most important key influencer (Fig 3). Respondents mentioned that all key influencers would likely (80% or more) recommend the government recommended childhood vaccination schedule profile (Fig 4). Except for the partner, Fig 5 shows that the gravid woman’s utility for government recommended childhood vaccination schedule profile changed a little bit due to the level of support for the government schedule she perceived from a certain key influencer. However, if there was a high consensus between key influencers (i.e. a high group support), the odds ratio impact for government recommended childhood vaccination schedule profile increased substantially (Fig 6) and showed the value of social consensus.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Average marginal impact of a key influencer on utility of a vaccination schedule.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276141.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Importance of key influencer views on respondent decision to vaccinate.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276141.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. How likely a respondent believes a certain key influencer would be to recommend each vaccination option.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276141.g004

thumbnail
Fig 5. Impact of key influencers on vaccination decision.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276141.g005

4. Discussion

Our study developed and empirically tested a choice model that accounted for social network influences in maternal choices for first year childhood vaccination in Australia. When the choice model took into account both childhood vaccination attributes and respondent beliefs about key influencer recommendations, our findings showed that the gravid woman’s decision for childhood vaccination was essentially almost completely socially driven. The probability of opting for the government recommended childhood vaccination schedule profile depended strongly on the gravid woman’s WHO vaccine hesitancy scale value, and on the level of social consensus between different key influencers. The marginal impact differed between key influencers, where the partner of the gravid woman had the largest impact on the maternal choice for childhood vaccination, followed by the gravid woman’s mother and friends with children. Having knowledge about the decision-makers’ social networks can change policy recommendations, since social networks generate significant influences on decision-makers. Namely, decision-makers exist in a matrix of social interdependence: health impact is not just on the individual but will potentially be felt throughout their social network.

In a qualitative study among MMR vaccination decisions, Poltorak et al. reported that actual choice outcomes depend not on a singular deliberative calculus and the information and education that informs it, but on contingent and unfolding personal and social circumstances in an evolving engagement [13]. This is completely in line with our quantitative results, which showed that the gravid woman’s WHO vaccine hesitancy scale value matters in childhood vaccination choices and that these choices are not made in a social vacuum at all. Like Brunson [49], our study contributed to the evidence that the value of social network consensus should not be underestimated in childhood vaccination decision-making. And more forcefully, our results suggest the existence of healthcare decisions (this first-year childhood vaccination schedule among them) which may be wholly driven by social influences rather than service attributes. Hence, to increase the uptake of the childhood vaccination it is more effective to convince the social network than to reduce costs or clinic visits, for example. The influence of a policymaker via the social network will work very strongly if one can reach and convince the partner and mother of the gravid woman as these are the two persons who are most efficacious increasing the mother’s likelihood to undertake childhood vaccination in Australia.

Given the authors’ prior experience with DCEs in health, marketing, transport and environmental economics, the findings that the gravid woman’s decision for childhood vaccination was essentially almost completely socially driven is an unusual result since theory would suggest that attributes are the strong drivers of choices. Instead, our DCE is demonstrating that social influences might be a strong explanator of behaviours in certain contexts. This observation should be strongly motivating for further research in social influences on individual choices, calling for further empirical work and choice paradigm evolution. That said, our study still showed that the DCE method is relevant here–even if the attributes of the vaccination were not the strongest drivers of stated choices for childhood vaccination in Australia—but also showed that DCE could benefit from extension. As shown in our study, this extension is needed to be able to jointly take health product/service/good attributes and social influences into account, while also considering the respondent’s trade-offs between the variables, between key influencers and between variables and key influencers. Taking the social context in DCEs into account is especially relevant when the research question focuses on emotionally costly and/or complex decisions, or where there may be strong social norms. This includes–for example–a wide range of physician-patient interactions involving serious health conditions, multiple physicians deciding on treatment and other types of group decision making.

Our DCE outcomes and the outcomes of the direct survey question regarding how important the view of a certain key influencer was to the decision-maker showed a reasonable level of convergent validity. That is, on the one hand, it showed that both methods led to the conclusion that the partner of the gravid woman had the largest impact on the maternal choice for childhood vaccination. On the other hand, it showed that who were the second and third important key influencers depend on the method used.

Although the choice model we developed in this study was able to account for social influences, we completely relied on key influencers preference information as perceived and reported by the gravid women only. Hensher et al. showed via their DCE study focusing on automobile purchase preferences, that sampling a single individual as a representative of the household’s preferences is less appropriate than using preference information from the relevant group of decision makers in the household [50]. Although individual preference is easier to be measured and less expensive to obtain, we recommend the future exploration of whether the conclusion of Hensher et al. holds for childhood vaccination and/or other health care decisions too.

The current focus on the individual decision-maker in health economics and beyond has created a gap in choice theory to account for social influences. At best, there exist econometric corrections for biases that might arise from (somewhat amorphous) social influences, conceptualized in the form of an “influence field” impacting the decision maker [14]. Such efforts do not constitute a theory of choice. To be useful for scientific purposes, a theory of individual choice in the presence of influencers needs to formulate specific hypotheses about specific mechanisms for the influence on the decision-maker. This gap in choice theory development has the further consequence that econometric models fall short of capturing interpretable social impacts on the decision-maker, leading to difficulties in interpretation of health policy implications. Finally, the lack of theory results in deficient and constrained measurement paradigms. Thus, despite the widespread recognition of social influences on individual choice behaviour in health care, and despite our and other choice modelers’ attempts [8, 14] to account for social influences, an overreliance on the extant microeconomic framework for choice focused on a unitary decision maker has perhaps stymied choice theory evolution. Hence, moving towards a socially interdependent choice paradigm in a rigorous manner is crucial to support and reach accurate ex-ante evaluation of health care policies.

Our study has several limitations that may hamper the generalizability of the results. First, the study used online and specialized panels, and the results were based on one medical context only. Further research is therefore needed to determine whether our results hold outside online panels and in other medical contexts, where weaker or stronger underlying social norms may exist. Second, due to practical constraints we included only women who were pregnant at the time of participating in the survey. Expanding data collection to include partners of pregnant women and collecting and analysing the data with and without consideration of joint decision-making would be ideal. Third, although a sample size of N = 604 is higher than an average DCE in healthcare [51] and is higher than Orme’s rule of thumb suggests [52], we could not avoid the mandatory aggregation of certain levels to achieve parameter identification and to estimate robust parameters. It might be that this study–like almost all quantitative studies—could have benefitted from a larger sample size to give more detailed outcomes. Finally, the study focused on the childhood vaccination for the first 12 months of a child’s life. As the role and influence of key influencers can change over time and with the age of the child, and parental motivators can change over time, assuming fixed individual’s childhood vaccination preferences over many years might not be realistic.

In conclusion, our DCE study showed that the maternal decision for childhood vaccination was essentially completely socially driven, suggesting that the potential impact of social network influences can and should be considered in health-related DCEs, especially if there are likely to be strong underlying social norms dictating decision maker behaviour.

Supporting information

S4 Appendix. Integrating multiple influencers into an ordered latent class choice model.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276141.s004

(DOCX)

S5 Appendix. Discrete choice experiment analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276141.s005

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank all respondents for their great contributions.

References

  1. 1. Bliemer M. & Rose J. Experimental design influences on stated choice outputs: an empirical study in air travel choice. Transp. Res. Part A Policy Pract. 45, 63–79 (2011).
  2. 2. Mahieu P.-A. et al. Stated preferences: a unique database composed of 1657 recent published articles in journals related to agriculture, environment, or health. Rev. Agric. Food Environ. Stud. (2017)
  3. 3. Luce R. D. & Tukey J. W. Simultaneous Conjoint-Measurement—a New Type of Fundamental Measurement. J. Math. Psychol. 1, 1–27 (1964).
  4. 4. Ryan M. & Gerard K. Using discrete choice experiments to value health care programmes: current practice and future research reflections. Applied Health Economics & Health Policy vol. 2 55–64 (2003). pmid:14619274
  5. 5. De Bekker-Grob E. W., Ryan M. & Gerard K. Discrete choice experiments in health economics: A review of the literature. Health Econ. 21, 145–172 (2012). pmid:22223558
  6. 6. Clark M., Determann D., Petrou S., Moro D. & de Bekker-Grob E. W. Discrete choice experiments in health economics: a review of the literature. Pharmacoeconomics 32, 883–902 (2014). pmid:25005924
  7. 7. Soekhai V., de Bekker-Grob E., Ellis A. & Vass C. Discrete Choice Experiments in Health Economics: Past, Present and Future. Pharmacoeconomics 37, 201–226 (2019). pmid:30392040
  8. 8. Adamowicz W. et al. Decision strategy and structure in households: A ‘groups’ perspective. Mark. Lett. (2005)
  9. 9. Determann D. et al. Public preferences for vaccination programmes during pandemics caused by pathogens transmitted through respiratory droplets–A discrete choice experiment in four European countries, 2013. Eurosurveillance (2016) pmid:27277581
  10. 10. Munro S. et al. Perspectives of patients, first-degree relatives and rheumatologists on preventive treatments for rheumatoid arthritis: A qualitative analysis. BMC Rheumatol. (2018) pmid:30886969
  11. 11. Flynn M. & Ogden J. Predicting uptake of MMR vaccination: A prospective questionnaire study. Br. J. Gen. Pract. (2004). pmid:15239915
  12. 12. Harmsen I. A. et al. Why parents refuse childhood vaccination: A qualitative study using online focus groups. BMC Public Health (2013) pmid:24341406
  13. 13. Poltorak M., Leach M., Fairhead J. & Cassell J. ‘MMR talk’ and vaccination choices: An ethnographic study in Brighton. Soc. Sci. Med. (2005) pmid:15899328
  14. 14. Eppstein M. J., Grover D. K., Marshall J. S. & Rizzo D. M. An agent-based model to study market penetration of plug-in hybrid electric vehicles. Energy Policy (2011)
  15. 15. Manca F., Sivakumar A., Daina N., Axsen J. & Polak J. W. Modelling the influence of peers’ attitudes on choice behaviour: Theory and empirical application on electric vehicle preferences. Transp. Res. Part A Policy Pract. (2020)
  16. 16. Attwell K. et al. Recent vaccine mandates in the United States, Europe and Australia: A comparative study. Vaccine (2018) pmid:30337171
  17. 17. Larson H. J., Jarrett C., Eckersberger E., Smith D. M. D. & Paterson P. Understanding vaccine hesitancy around vaccines and vaccination from a global perspective: A systematic review of published literature, 2007–2012. Vaccine (2014) pmid:24598724
  18. 18. De Bekker-Grob E. W. et al. Labeled versus unlabeled discrete choice experiments in health economics: An application to colorectal cancer screening. Value Heal. 13, (2010). pmid:19912597
  19. 19. Dubé E., Vivion M. & MacDonald N. E. Vaccine hesitancy, vaccine refusal and the anti-vaccine movement: Influence, impact and implications. Expert Review of Vaccines (2014) pmid:25373435
  20. 20. Dubé E. et al. Vaccine hesitancy: An overview. Human Vaccines and Immunotherapeutics (2013) pmid:23584253
  21. 21. Bair R. M. et al. Acceptability to Latino Parents of Sexually Transmitted Infection Vaccination. Ambul. Pediatr. (2008) pmid:18355738
  22. 22. Bishai D., Brice R., Girod I., Saleh A. & Ehreth J. Conjoint analysis of French and German parents’ willingness to pay for meningococcal vaccine. PharmacoEconomics (2007) pmid:17249856
  23. 23. Oteng B. et al. Evaluating societal preferences for human papillomavirus vaccine and cervical smear test screening programme. Sex. Transm. Infect. (2011) pmid:20956352
  24. 24. Poulos C. et al. Mothers’ preferences and willingness to pay for HPV vaccines in Vinh Long Province, Vietnam. Soc. Sci. Med. (2011) pmid:21733609
  25. 25. Sadique M. Z., Devlin N., Edmunds W. J. & Parkin D. The Effect of Perceived Risks on the Demand for Vaccination: Results from a Discrete Choice Experiment. PLoS One (2013) pmid:23408936
  26. 26. Shono A. & Kondo M. Factors associated with seasonal influenza vaccine uptake among children in Japan. BMC Infect. Dis. (2015) pmid:25886607
  27. 27. Brown D. S., Johnson F. R., Poulos C. & Messonnier M. L. Mothers’ preferences and willingness to pay for vaccinating daughters against human papillomavirus. Vaccine (2010) pmid:20044060
  28. 28. de Bekker-Grob E. W. et al. Girls’ preferences for HPV vaccination: A discrete choice experiment. Vaccine (2010) pmid:20708696
  29. 29. Hofman R. et al. Parents’ preferences for vaccinating daughters against human papillomavirus in the Netherlands: A discrete choice experiment. BMC Public Health (2014) pmid:24885861
  30. 30. Flood E. M. et al. Parent preferences for pediatric influenza vaccine attributes. Clin. Pediatr. (Phila). (2011) pmid:21196417
  31. 31. Flood E. M. et al. A survey of children’s preferences for influenza vaccine attributes. Vaccine (2011) pmid:21510993
  32. 32. Gidengil C. et al. Parental and societal values for the risks and benefits of childhood combination vaccines. Vaccine (2012) pmid:22449423
  33. 33. Hall J. et al. Using stated preference discrete choice modelling to evaluate the introduction of varicella vaccination. Health Econ. (2002) pmid:12112494
  34. 34. Muangchana C. & Bishai D. The private demand for hib vaccination in a probable low hib disease incidence country: Thailand 2006. Southeast Asian J. Trop. Med. Public Health (2010).
  35. 35. Johnson F. R. et al. Constructing experimental designs for discrete-choice experiments: Report of the ISPOR conjoint analysis experimental design good research practices task force. Value Heal. (2013) pmid:23337210
  36. 36. ChoiceMetrics. Ngene 1.1.1 User Manual & Reference Guide, Australia. (2012).
  37. 37. Zingg A. & Siegrist M. Measuring people’s knowledge about vaccination: Developing a one-dimensional scale. Vaccine (2012) pmid:22445808
  38. 38. Larson H. J. et al. Measuring vaccine hesitancy: The development of a survey tool the SAGE Working Group on Vaccine Hesitancy. Vaccine (2015).
  39. 39. Holt C. L. et al. Development and validation of an instrument to assess perceived social influence on health behaviors. J. Health Psychol. (2010) pmid:20522506
  40. 40. Lancsar E. & Louviere J. Conducting discrete choice experiments to inform healthcare decision making: A user’s guide. PharmacoEconomics vol. 26 661–677 (2008). pmid:18620460
  41. 41. Lancsar E., Fiebig D. G. & Hole A. R. Discrete Choice Experiments: A Guide to Model Specification, Estimation and Software. Pharmacoeconomics 35, 697–716 (2017). pmid:28374325
  42. 42. Hauber A. B. et al. Statistical Methods for the Analysis of Discrete Choice Experiments: A Report of the ISPOR Conjoint Analysis Good Research Practices Task Force. Value Heal. 19, 300–315 (2016).
  43. 43. Swait J. & Marley A. A. J. Probabilistic choice (models) as a result of balancing multiple goals. J. Math. Psychol. (2013)
  44. 44. Swait J. & Adamowicz W. The influence of task complexity on consumer choice: A latent class model of decision strategy switching. J. Consum. Res. (2001)
  45. 45. Gopinath & Ben-Akiva. Estimation of Randomly Distributed Value of Time. (1995).
  46. 46. Swait J. D. & Sweeney J. Perceived Value and Its Impact on Choice Behavior in a Retail Setting. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 7, 77–88 (2000).
  47. 47. Swait J. D. & Adamowicz W. The Influence of Task Complexity on Consumer Choice: A Latent Class Model of Decision Strategy Switching. J. Consum. Res. 28, 135–148 (2001).
  48. 48. Huls S. P. I. et al. Preference variation: where does health-risk attitude come into the equation? Value Heal. (2022). pmid:35750590
  49. 49. Brunson E. K. The impact of social networks on parents’ vaccination decisions. Pediatrics (2013) pmid:23589813
  50. 50. Hensher D. A., Beck M. J. & Rose J. M. Accounting for Preference and Scale Heterogeneity in Establishing Whether it Matters Who is Interviewed to Reveal Household Automobile Purchase Preferences. Environ. Resour. Econ. (2011)
  51. 51. Watson V, Becker F. & de Bekker-Grob E. Discrete Choice Experiment Response Rates: A Meta-analysis. Health Econ. 26, 810–817 (2017). pmid:27122445
  52. 52. Orme B. Sample Size Issues for Conjoint Analysis, in Getting Started with Conjoint Analysis: Strategies for Product Design and Pricing Research. (2010).