Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Reaching skills in six-month-old infants at environmental and biological risk

  • Liz Araújo Rohr ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    lizrohr.nenem@gmail.com

    Affiliation Department of Physical Therapy, Federal University of São Carlos, São Carlos, São Paulo, Brazil

  • Thais Invenção Cabral,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio, United States of America

  • Murilo Mageste de Moraes,

    Roles Data curation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Physical Therapy, Federal University of São Carlos, São Carlos, São Paulo, Brazil

  • Eloisa Tudella

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Physical Therapy, Federal University of São Carlos, São Carlos, São Paulo, Brazil

Abstract

Objectives

To assess kinematic parameters and proximal and distal reaching adjustments of infants at biological or environmental risk and compare with reaching performance of six-month-old full-term infants without known risk factors.

Methods

This blinded cross-sectional study included 62 infants at six months of age divided into three independent groups: Group with no known risk factor (NRF), 28 full-terms with no risk factors; Low SES group (LSES):19 full-terms classified as low socioeconomic status and no biological risk; Very preterm group (VPT), 15 very preterm infants at six months corrected age and no environmental risk. Infants were placed in a reclined baby chair at 45°, and a malleable and unfamiliar object was presented to the infant at 5-second intervals to elicit reaching movements.

Results

Infants from LSES presented reaching duration (p = 0.032, Cohen’s f = 0.349) and movement unit (p = 0.033, Cohen’s f = 0.351) significantly higher than VPT group. Horizontal hand orientation was moderately associated with infants at environmental risk (p = 0.031; Cramer’s V = 0.30).

Conclusion

Infants of low socioeconomic status perform less functional reaching movements than very preterm infants at six months corrected age. Socioeconomic status may impact more on reaching skills than biological risk. Given the importance of reaching for infant development, low-cost public health strategies are needed to identify possible delays.

Introduction

Reaching is defined as the ability to locate and stare at an object and stretch out one or both hands toward its direction to touch or grasp it [1,2]. This skill emerges in full-term infants approximately at 3–4 months of age [35], characterizing one of the first stages of voluntary motor development [6]. Reaching also allows exploration and manipulation of objects in the environment.

This ability can be assessed in detail using kinematic analysis [7] and a set of variables that provide important information regarding quality of movement, such as straightness (straightness index) and smoothness (movement unit) [8,9]. Studies involving kinematic analysis showed that upper limb movement paths are more curved during reaching acquisition, with variations in speed, movement units, and duration [3,812]. Efficient reaching is achieved at six-month-old [13,14], and it becomes more fluent and straight according to experience and practice. Infants first perform bimanual reaching (proximal adjustment) [15] with hands in horizontal orientation (distal adjustment) [16] and improve this skill by performing unimanual reaching with the hand open and oriented vertically [16].

Infant development occurs in a bidirectional and non-linear way, according to interactions between personal and environmental factors, body functions and structures, and activities and participation [17]. Thus, risk factors, such as biological (prematurity, neonatal hypoxemia, or low birth weight) or environmental (family factors, environmental structure, socioeconomic status [SES], or parental educational level) [18], may negatively impact reaching acquisition and engagement in activities and participation.

Infants at biological risk and late preterm infants present less functional kinematic parameters, slower movements, increased deceleration time, and non-fluid and immature reaching behavior compared with full-terms [19,20]. On the other hand, very preterm infants perform similar movement frequency and units than healthy full-term infants, indicating efficient reaching tasks [21]. However, these infants use bimanual strategies more often and present more curved reaching paths than full-term infants. Furthermore, reaching in very preterm infants is associated with development of other areas (e.g., language and cognition) and influenced by frequency of bimanual strategies, differing from the impact of kinematic parameters (e.g., straightness index and movement unit) observed in extremely preterm infants [22].

Evidence regarding reaching development in infants at environmental risk is scarce. The study conducted by Cabral (2017) [23] observed lower reaching performance (i.e., higher deceleration index) in orphaned infants than non-orphaned healthy infants. Characteristics of physical environments (i.e., home and social environments) are important for development and contribute to interaction and personal interest of the infant [24]. Clearfild et al., (2014) [25] identified delayed exploratory behavior (transfer and spin) in infants with low SES compared with infants with high SES, probably due to reduced repertoire of fine motor skills and environment with limited stimuli. Thus, environments with few affordances and opportunities for exploration may be unfavorable for developing fine motor skills, such as manual reach.

The literature regarding reaching skills in infants at risk is still scanty and inconsistent, and associations between infant reaching skills and risk factors are not clear. In this context, it is necessary to understand the development of these skills in infants exposed to different risk conditions since environmental and biological risks may interfere with skills development and maturation. Thus, this study aimed to identify differences in kinematic parameters and proximal and distal reaching adjustments in infants at biological risk (very preterm) and environmental risk (low SES) and compare reaching performance with six-month-old full-term infants without risk factors. We hypothesized that infants at biological and environmental risk would be less functional and present similar kinematic parameters than infants without risk.

Methods

Ethical procedures

This blinded cross-sectional study was approved by the research ethics committee of the Federal University of São Carlos (UFSCar) (Brazil) (no. 79741917.8.0000.5504), and followed the resolution 466/12 of the National Health Council and the Declaration of Helsinki. The parents/caregivers of the infants gave written informed consent.

Participants and eligibility criteria

Full-term infants (≥ 37 gestational weeks) at six months of age and very preterm infants (28 to 32 gestational weeks) at six months corrected age of both genders were eligible for the study [26]. Corrected age was calculated by subtracting the number of weeks the infant was born before 40 weeks of gestation from the infant’s chronological age. Infants without signs of neurological impairment, congenital abnormalities in the central nervous system, musculoskeletal disorders, genetic syndromes, congenital infections, sensory deficits, and whose parents accepted to participate by signing the informed consent form were included.

Poverty income ratio (PIR) and maternal educational level determined SES [27,28]. PIR is the ratio of household income to the poverty level specific to family composition and geographic area and can be classified as high SES (maternal education more than high school and PIR ≥ 2), middle SES (high school education regardless of PIR value), and low SES (maternal education less than high school and PIR < 2) [27,28].

Recruitment.

All infants were recruited from the maternity hospital at Santa Casa de Misericórdia in the city of São Carlos (Brazil). After identification, parents/caregivers of infants were contacted by telephone and invited to participate in the study. Three hundred forty-seven telephone calls were performed: 170 did not answer, 38 were not interested in participating, and seven did not live in the city. Of those who agreed to participate, 44 did not attend the assessment, 16 did not meet eligibility criteria and 8 were excluded due to constant crying during evaluation. Also, two infants classified as low SES were excluded from final analysis because they did not perform reaching movements during assessments. All parents/caregivers voluntarily participated in the study, and displacement was performed without costs.

The study included 62 six-month-old infants from both genders (51.61% males) divided into three independent groups: no known risk factor (NRF): 28 full-terms without known risk factors; Low SES group (LSES): 19 full-terms classified as low SES with no biological risks; Very preterm group (VPT): 15 very preterm infants at six months corrected age and without low SES Table 1.

Procedures

First, a questionnaire developed by the authors and composed of birth, personal, and sociodemographic information was applied to parents to identify environmental and biological risks and characterize infants (S1 and S2 Files). Right after, reaching movements were analyzed quantitatively and qualitatively. After the assessment, all caregivers received a booklet containing guidance on how to stimulate infant development.

Assessment of reaching.

A three-dimensional motion capture system (Qualysis, Gothenburg, Sweden) composed of five stroboscopic cameras assessed kinematic variables using a frequency of 200 Hz. X, Y, and Z coordinates were considered in the sagittal (anteroposterior), frontal (mediolateral), and longitudinal (superior-inferior) planes, respectively (Fig 1). Four spherical passive markers were positioned on the infant’s head, right and left wrists, and trunk with double-sided hypoallergenic tape. A fifth maker was placed on a malleable object unfamiliar to the infant (Fig 2).

thumbnail
Fig 1. XYZ coordinates and markers.

Markers: Yellow: Head; pink: Right wrist; purple: Left wrist; blue: Trunk.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254106.g001

Reaching of all infants was assessed by the same researcher, positioned in front of the infant [19,29]. Infants were placed in a baby chair reclined 45° from the horizontal, and the researcher always presented the object in the midline (shoulder height) of the infant. After reaching, the object was taken away and presented again at 5-second intervals. The researcher continued performing these movements to avoid habituation even if infants did not initiate a new reaching movement. This procedure was performed for one minute.

Only successful reaching movements were analyzed. A reach was considered successful when one or two hands touched the object, whether followed by grasping or not [13,30]. The beginning of a reach was defined as the first frame where the arm initiated an uninterrupted movement toward the object, deviating from middle waistline. The end of a reach was defined as the first frame the infant touched the object [3,3033].

Kinematic variables.

The following spatio-temporal variables were analyzed: 1) straightness index (i.e., straightness of the reach), calculated as the ratio between the shortest distance that could have been traveled to reach the object and the actual distance traveled by the hand. A value of one represented a straight approach [8,9]; 2) reaching duration, calculated as the time between the beginning and end of the movement [8,9]; 3) movement unit (i.e., the number of decelerations and accelerations used to correct the trajectory), calculated as the number of maximum velocities between two minimum velocities, where the difference exceeded one cm/s(2)—the lower the movement unit, the smoother the reaching movement [7]; 4) mean velocity, obtained as the ratio between hand displacement and reaching duration [8,9,29,34] 5) peak velocity, corresponding to the highest velocity achieved during the movement [2]; 6) deceleration time (time taken to decelerate the movement), calculated as the time between peak velocity and the end of the reach [8,19].

Qualitative analysis of reaching.

Frequency of reaches was calculated as the number of successful reaching movements over one minute. Quality of reaching was assessed through proximal (uni- or bimanual) and distal adjustments when the hand of the infant touched the object.

Proximal adjustments. Proximal reaching adjustments indicates whether reaching was performed with one or both hands and were classified into: a) unimanual, when reach was accomplished with one hand [29,30]; b) bimanual, when reach was accomplished with both hands simultaneously toward the object and touched it, or when hands reached the object with a difference of ≤ 67 frames (frames per second) [29,30].

Distal adjustments. Distal adjustments refer to hand position when touching the object. The following distal adjustments were considered: a) hand orientation, corresponding to hand position when the infant touched the object and classified as: horizontal (palm faced downwards with forearm pronated), vertical (palm oriented toward the infant’s midline with forearm in neutral position), and oblique (hand in an intermediary position between horizontal and vertical positions) [16,30]; b) hand opening, referred to the position of fingers at the touching moment and classified as: open (fingers fully extended or slightly flexed), closed (fingers completely flexed), and semiopen (fingers between open and closed positions) [35]; c) hand and fingers surface area of contact, indicating whether the touch was performed with the ventral or dorsal region of the hand.

Two blinded researchers were responsible for data processing. Kinematic data processing was performed using customized Matlab® 9.2 scripts, with a 4th-order Butterworth filter and cut-off frequency of 6Hz. Assessment of proximal and distal adjustments was recorded by a camera, and qualitative analyses were performed using Kinovea 0.8.21.

Inter-rater reliability

Cohen’s Kappa evaluated inter-rater reliability in 20% of the infants. Inter-rater reliability was 0.74 (moderate, p<0.001) for proximal adjustments, 0.87 (strong, p<0.001) for hand orientation, 0.83 (strong, p<0.001) for hand and fingers opening, and 0.78 (moderate, p<0.001) for hand and fingers surface area of contact.

Statistical analysis

Sample size was calculated (G*power software, v.3.1.9.2, Kiel, Germany) using mean and standard deviation of reaching frequency of a pilot study conducted with 18 participants. With an effect size of 0.52, 80% statistical power, and alpha value of 0.05, a minimum of 39 participants (i.e., 13 participants per group) was estimated.

Shapiro-Wilk and Levene’s test verified data normality and homogeneity of variance, respectively. Frequency of reaches, mean velocity, peak velocity, movement unit, and deceleration time were averaged and compared between groups using ordinary one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA), followed by Tukey’s post hoc. Straightness index was compared between groups using Kruskal-Wallis test, followed by Dunn’s post hoc.

According to data normality, effect sizes were calculated for all data using Cohen’s f or epsilon-squared (ε2) to avoid type II error. The former was interpreted as small (Cohen’s f = 0.10), medium (Cohen’s f = 0.25), and large (Cohen’s f = 0.40) [36], while the latter comprised coefficient values between 0 (no relationship) and 1 (perfect relationship) [37]. Relationships between kinematic variables in all groups were performed using Pearson’s correlation coefficient (r) and interpreted as weak (0.10–0.39), moderate (0.40–0.69), strong (0.70–0.89), and very strong (0.90–1.00) [37].

Associations between risk factors (environmental and biological) and proximal (unimanual and bimanual) and distal adjustments (hand orientation, hand opening, and hand and fingers surface area of contact) were verified using Chi-squared test. Cramer’s V assessed the strength of the association and was interpreted as weak (<0.299), moderate (0.300–0.499), and strong (>0.500) [38].

All statistical analyses were performed using the Statistical Package Social Science software version 22 (IBM. Corp.®, EUA), and the significance level was set at α = 0.05 (two-tailed).

Results

Three hundred fifty-nine reaching movements were performed, of which 305 were considered valid for analysis. Regarding kinematic variables, four reaches were excluded during data processing; thus, 301 reaches were analyzed.

The frequency of reach was higher in the NRF group (5.36 ± 2.36) than LSES group (4.17 ± 2.31) and VPT group (5.33 ± 2.72); however, this difference was not statistically significant (p = 0.141). Reaching duration (p = 0.032, Cohen’s f = 0.349) and movement unit (p = 0.033, Cohen’s f = 0.351) were significantly higher in LSES group than VPT group (Fig 3). Conversely, frequency of reach (p = 0.141), straightness index (0.401), mean velocity (p = 0.539), peak velocity (p = 0.266), and deceleration time (p = 280) were not different between groups Table 2.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Reaching duration (A) and movement unit (B) between groups.

Center lines indicate median and plus signs show mean values. Dots represent each infant. Upper and lower limits of each box represent the 75th and 25th percentiles, respectively. Whiskers denote minimum and maximum values. cm/s: Centimeter per second. Significance is represented by post hoc p-values.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254106.g003

thumbnail
Table 2. Mean, standard deviation, and interquartile range for kinematic variables in each group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254106.t002

A weak correlation between straightness index and frequency of reach (r = 0.382; p = 0.044) and a moderate correlation between movement unit and reaching duration (r = 0.640; p = 0.0002) were found in infants from G1. Regarding infants from LSES group, only a weak correlation between movement unit and reaching duration (r = 0.813; p < 0.0001) were observed. Conversely, a moderate correlation between movement unit and reaching duration (r = 0.578, p = 0.0001) and a strong correlation between movement unit and deceleration time (r = 0.763, p = 0.0009) were identified in infants from VPT group Table 3.

thumbnail
Table 3. Correlation coefficient between kinematic variables in each group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254106.t003

A moderate association (Cramer’s V = 0.30; p = 0.031) between hand orientation (hand in horizontal orientation) and factor Group (infants from LSES group) was also observed Table 4.

Discussion

This study aimed to verify differences in kinematic parameters and proximal and distal reaching adjustments between very preterm infants, infants of low SES, and infants without known risk factors. Our hypothesis that infants at biological and environmental risk would present similar kinematic parameters was not confirmed. Infants with low SES presented less efficient reaching with higher movement units and reaching duration than very preterm infants, refuting our hypothesis.

To our knowledge, this was the first study comparing reaching performance between infants at different risk conditions. Furthermore, understanding kinematic variables that best describe reaching movement in these groups and identifying what would be expected to achieve mature and efficient reaching enhances therapists’ strategies in early intervention programs.

Reaching is a fundamental skill for development. Infants explore and interact with environment through reaching movements, facilitating cognitive, perceptual, and social development [2,6,30,39]. Therefore, infants with low SES and impaired reaching may negatively impact other areas of development, resulting in limited exploration and social participation repertoires.

Data indicated that all kinematic parameters were less functional in the LSES group than NRF and VPT group. However, only movement unit and reaching duration were different between LSES group and VPT group. Compared with other infants, infants of low SES performed more sudden movements (movement unit) and spent more time (reaching duration) to complete the task, indicating less control and more time needed to finish reaching movements. Thus, we believe that these infants present a less efficient reach than very preterm infants.

Interestingly, relationships between movement unit and reaching duration were found in all groups, suggesting that these variables indicate reaching efficiency since the more fluent (movement unit) the reach, the less the time needed to perform the movement (movement duration). Relationships between straightness index and frequency of reach in infants from NRF group indicated that the straighter the movement, the fewer the corrections needed in the trajectory, favoring reaching frequency. Kinematic analysis of reaching movements is complex, and its analysis provides a better understanding of movement efficiency.

Regarding proximal and distal adjustments, only hand orientation (horizontal) associated significantly with infants from LSES group. This result suggests that infants of low SES are more prone to perform reaching movements with hands in the horizontal position, considered an immature adjustment [16,30]. This also corroborates with kinematic findings, indicating that strategies adopted to perform reaching movements are less functional and may negatively impact grasping and object manipulation.

Our results contrast those from Greco (2020) [40], who applied reaching-specific training or social training on the emergence of reaching in infants of low SES. Infants were evaluated after five days and when completed six months of age. The author observed improvements in kinematic variables and proximal and distal adjustments, regardless of training type, suggesting that infants of low SES present adequate functional reaching at six months of age.

The environment seems to play an important role in the development of reaching skills. We believe that context in which infants of low SES live may hinder opportunities to practice reaching skills and manipulate different objects and toys. This would explain why their strategies were less functional. Previous studies have shown that environments with adequate stimuli contribute positively to motor performance [41,42]. Brain growth and development are also influenced by environmental factors [43] since worse socioeconomic status may impact brain activity of infants, impairing language, attention [44], hippocampus, and amygdala development [45].

Our results differ from literature regarding preterm infants. At six and seven months corrected age, reaching movements of preterm infants are immature, curved, and slower than full-term infants [19,20]. Although bimanual reaching, curved trajectories, and compensatory movements are more frequent at eight months corrected age, frequency of reaching and movement units are similar to full-term infants [21].

Kinematic parameters of very preterm infants were not different from NRF infants. Reaching was considered straight, smooth, with movement speed and duration similar to infants in the NRF. We believe the results differed from literature because participants in our study did not present associated risk factors (i.e., the same infant was not classified as preterm and low SES). Conversely, this information is unclear in previous studies [1921]. In our study, the researcher presented the object in the infant’s midline, differing from Grönqvist et al. (2011) [21], who probably influenced task demand using moving toys to elicit reaching movements. Also, at six months corrected age, very preterm infants presented appropriate biological factors (i.e., weight and length). Thus, the lack of environmental risk (i.e., low SES and maternal educational level) may have helped proper reaching development. Furthermore, infants were assessed at six months corrected age, indicating temporal advantage over other infants due to greater extra-uterine opportunities to practice the skill [46].

Lastly, no significant differences were found between infants at risk and without risk. We investigated reaching performance in infants with specific risk factors, such as SES and prematurity, and SES classification considered important environmental factors (maternal education, income, and number of people living in the house). Although infants did not present associated risk factors (i.e., low SES and very preterm birth), we did not use any instrument (e.g., Affordances in the Home Environment for Motor Development—Infant Scale) [47] to assess quantity and quality of stimuli received in the family environment. Affordances provided by the environment may play an important role in motor and cognitive development [48]. Thus, further studies are needed to investigate the influence of quality of affordances on development of reaching of infants with and without risks and explore relationships between development of reaching and other fine motor skills, such as manual exploration.

Limitations

This study has limitations that should be considered. First, monitoring of reach skills was not conducted since its emergence; therefore, it is unknown whether infants of low SES presented delayed skill acquisition. Second, although none of the included infants attended rehabilitation programs, results may have been influenced by caregivers’ awareness regarding special needs, leading to information search and assistance in specialized healthcare centers. Third, our study did not include infants with more than one risk factor or evaluated house stimuli and environmental variables. Thus, future studies should emphasize premature infants with low SES and verify the role of environmental variables in early motor development since low SES may be associated with an unfavorable amount of environmental stimuli.

Clinical implications

Our findings highlight important evidence about reaching performance in six-month-old infants at biological and environmental risk. SES seems to impact skill performance more than biological risk when these infants present no risk factors associated. The results of this study do not minimize findings regarding infants at biological risk [33,4952]; however, we emphasize the importance of low-cost strategies (i.e., stimulation strategies for families and educators) within the scope of public health. A closer approach to the development of infants of low SES may reduce its impacts on future life. According to Bornstein et al. [53], infants at five months of age with advanced motor-exploratory competence achieve higher cognitive development at four and 10 years and higher academic levels at 10 and 14 years.

Conclusions

At six months of age, infants of low SES perform reaching movements with less smooth trajectories than very preterm infants at six months corrected age. Also, infants of low SES are more prone to reach objects with hands in horizontal position, which is not functional for the age. Therefore, greater attention is needed for this group, given the importance of reaching for infant motor development and the lack of early intervention programs. Low-cost strategies to encourage development of reaching skills and minimize negative functional impacts of environmental factors must be investigated in future studies.

Supporting information

S1 File. Sociodemographic questionnaire.

English version.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254106.s001

(DOCX)

S2 File. Sociodemographic questionnaire.

Portuguese version.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254106.s002

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Probatus Academic Services for providing scientific language translation, revision, and editing.

References

  1. 1. Savelsbergh GJP, van der Kamp J. The effect of body orientation to gravity on early infant reaching. Vol. 58, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1994: 510–28. pmid:7844502
  2. 2. Thelen E, Corbetta D, Spencer JP. Development of Reaching during the First Year: Role of Movement Speed. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1996;22(5):1059–76. pmid:8865616
  3. 3. Cunha AB, Woollacott M, Tudella E. Influence of specific training on spatio-temporal parameters at the onset of goal-directed reaching in infants: A controlled trial. Brazilian J Phys Ther. 2013;17(4):409–17. Avaiable from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1413-35552012005000099.
  4. 4. von Hofsten C. Development of visually directed reaching: the approach phase. Journal of Human Movement Studies.1979; 5: 160–178.
  5. 5. Thelen E, Corbetta D, Kamm K, Spencer JP, Schneider K, Zernicke RF. The Transition to Reaching: Mapping Intention and Intrinsic Dynamics. Child Dev. 1993;64(4):1058–98. pmid:8404257
  6. 6. Fallang B, Saugstad OD, Grøgaard J, Hadders-Algra M. Kinematic quality of reaching movements in preterm infants. Pediatr Res. 2003;53(5):836–42. pmid:12612201
  7. 7. Lima CD, Carvalho RP, Barros RML, Tudella E. Two different methods for kinematic analysis of head movements relating to eye-head coordination in infants. Rev Bras Fisioter. 2008; 12(5); 425–31. Avaiable from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1413-35552008000500013.
  8. 8. Carvalho RP, Tudella E, Savelsbergh GJP. Spatio-temporal parameters in infant’s reaching movements are influenced by body orientation. Infant Behav Dev. 2007;30(1):26–35. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.07.006. pmid:17292777
  9. 9. von Hofsten C. Structuring of early reaching movements: A longitudinal study. J Mot Behav. 1991;23(4):280–92. Avaiable from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00222895.1991.9942039. pmid:14766510
  10. 10. de Graaf-Peters VB, Bakker H, Van Eykern LA, Otten B, Hadders-Algra M. Postural adjustments and reaching in 4- and 6-month-old infants: An EMG and kinematical study. Exp Brain Res. 2007;181(4):647–56. Avaiable from: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-0964-6. pmid:17505820
  11. 11. Fallang B, Saugstad OD, Hadders-Algra M. Goal directed reaching and postural control in supine position in healthy infants. Behav Brain Res. 2000;115(1):9–18. Avaiable from: https://doi.org/10.1016/s0166-4328(00)00231-x. pmid:10996403
  12. 12. Cunha AB, Soares DA, Carvalho RP, Rosander K, von Hofsten C, Tudella E. Maturational and situational determinants of reaching at its onset. Infant Behav Dev. 2015;41:64–72. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.06.003. pmid:26311469
  13. 13. Bakker H, de Graaf-Peters VB, van Eykern LA, Otten B, Hadders-Algra M. Development of proximal arm muscle control during reaching in young infants: From variation to selection. Infant Behav Dev. 2010;33(1):30–8. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.10.006. pmid:19945169
  14. 14. Toledo, AM.; Soares-Marangoni, DA.; Fonseca, MV.; Tudella, E. Introdução ao alcance manual de lactentes. In: Tudella, E.; Toledo, A.M.; Lima-Alvarez, C.D. (org). Intervenção precoce evidências para a prática clínica em lactentes de risco. 1aEd., Appris. 2019:155–170.
  15. 15. Fagard J, Lockman JJ. The effect of task constraints on infants’ (bi)manual strategy for grasping and exploring objects. Infant Behav Dev. 2005;28:305–15. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.05.005.
  16. 16. Fagard J. Linked proximal and distal changes in the reaching behavior of 5-to 12 month-old human infants grasping objects of different sizes. Infant Behav Dev. 2000;23(3/4):317–29.
  17. 17. Organização Mundial da Saúde (OMS). CIF-CJ: Classificação Internacional de Funcionalidade, Incapacidade e Saúde: versão para crianças e jovens. São Paulo: Editora da Universidade Federal de São Carlos; 2011. 312 p.
  18. 18. King EH, Logsdon DA, Schroeder SR. Risk Factors for Developmental Delay Among Infants and Toddlers. Child Heal Care. 1992;21(1):39–52. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15326888chc2101_6. pmid:10145594
  19. 19. Toledo AM, Tudella E. The development of reaching behavior in low-risk preterm infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2008;31(3):398–407. Available from: https://10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.12.006%2031. pmid:18289691
  20. 20. Sato NTDS, Tudella E. Influence of sitting positions and level of trunk control during reaching movements in late preterm and full-term infants. Front Pediatr. 2018;6:1–11. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fped.2018.00185.
  21. 21. Grönqvist H, Strand BK, von Hofsten C. Reaching strategies of very preterm infants at 8 months corrected age. Exp Brain Res. 2011;209(2):225–33. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2538-x. pmid:21267552
  22. 22. Kaul YF, Rosander K, Grönqvist H, Strand BK, Hellström-Westas L, von Hofsten C. Reaching skills of infants born very preterm predict neurodevelopment at 2.5 years. Infant Behav Dev. 2019;57(May):101333. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2019.101333. pmid:31238256
  23. 23. Cabral TI. Proposta de intervenção motora e treino de alcance manual para a promoção do desenvolvimento motor e do alcance manual em lactentes abrigados [Thesis]. São Carlos: Universidade Federal de São Carlos; 2017. 96 p.
  24. 24. Bronfenbrenner U, Morris P. The ecology of developmental processes. In: Damon W. (Org.) Handbook of child psychology. New York: John Wiley & Sons. 1998.
  25. 25. Clearfield MW, Bailey LS, Jenne HK, Stanger SB, & Tacke N. Socioeconomic status affects oral and manual exploration across the first year. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2014;35(1): 63–69. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21423. pmid:25424407
  26. 26. Organização Mundial da Saúde (OMS). Nacimientos prematuros. 2016. http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs363/es/.
  27. 27. Cunha AB, Babik I, Ross SM, Logan SW, Galloway JC, Clary E, et al. Prematurity may negatively impact means-end problem solving across the first two years of life. Res Dev Disabil. 2018;81:24–36. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2018.03.007. pmid:29609836
  28. 28. Karlamangla AS, Merkin SS, Crimmins EM, Seeman TE. Socioeconomic and Ethnic Disparities in Cardiovascular Risk In the United States, 2001–2006. Ann Epidemiol. 2010;20(8):617–28. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.annepidem.2010.05.003. pmid:20609342
  29. 29. Corbetta D, Thelen E, Johnson K. Motor constraints on the development of perception-action matching in infant reaching. Infant Behav Dev. 2000;23(3):351–74. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00049-2.
  30. 30. Toledo AM, Soares DA, Tudella E. Proximal and distal adjustments of reaching behavior in preterm infants. J Mot Behav. 2011;43(2):137–45. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2011.552076. pmid:21400327
  31. 31. Carvalho RP, Tudella E, Caljouw SR, Savelsbergh GJP. Early control of reaching: Effects of experience and body orientation. Infant Behav Dev. 2008;31(1):23–33. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.06.001. pmid:17658609
  32. 32. Cunha AB, Soares DA, Ferro AM, Tudella E. Effect of training at different body positions on proximal and distal reaching adjustments at the onset of goal-directed reaching: A controlled clinical trial. Motor Control. 2013;17(2):123–44. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/mcj.17.2.123. pmid:23579561
  33. 33. Soares DA, van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJ, Tudella E. The effect of a short bout of practice on reaching behavior in late preterm infants at the onset of reaching: A randomized controlled trial. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34(12):4546–58. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.09.028. pmid:24145045
  34. 34. Mathew A, Cook M. The Control of Reaching Movements by Young Infants Mathew and Cook 1239. Child Dev. 1990;61(4):1238–57.
  35. 35. Guimarães EL, Cunha AB, Soares DA, Tudella E. Reaching behavior in preterm infants during the first year of life: A systematic review. Motor Control. 2013;17(4):340–54. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/mcj.17.4.340. pmid:23756345
  36. 36. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, N.J.: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1988. 1988 p.
  37. 37. Tomczak M, Tomczak E. The need to report effect size estimates revisited. An overview of some recommended measures of effect size. Trends Sport Sci. 2014;1(21):19–25.
  38. 38. Cohen J. A power primer. Vol. 112. Psychol Bull; 1992. p. 155–9.
  39. 39. Libertus K and Needham A. Reaching experience increases face preference in 3-month-old infants. Developmental Science. 2011;14(6):1355–64. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01084.x. pmid:22010895
  40. 40. Greco ALR. Efeito do treino precoce com luvas abertas aderentes no aprimoramento do alcance de lactentes de baixo nível socioeconômico: ensaio clínico randomizado. [Thesis]. São Carlos: Universidade Federal de São Carlos; 2020. 127 p.
  41. 41. Hua J, Duan T, Gu G, Wo D, Zhu Q, Liu JQ, et al. Effects of home and education environments on children’s motor performance in China. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2016;58(8):868–76. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/dmcn.13073. pmid:26893014
  42. 42. Swann HE, Brumley MR. Locomotion and Posture Development in Immature Male and Female Rats (Rattus norvegicus): Comparison of Sensory- Enriched versus Sensory-Deprived Testing Environments. Physiol Behav. 2019;133(2):183–196. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/com0000147. pmid:30382711
  43. 43. Halperin JM. Influence of environmental enrichment, cognitive enhancement adn PE on brain development(ADHD). Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(3):621–34. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.006.
  44. 44. Tomalski P, Moore DG, Ribeiro H, Axelsson EL, Murphy E, Karmiloff-Smith A, et al. Socioeconomic status and functional brain development—associations in early infancy. Dev Sci. 2013;16(5):676–87. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.12079. pmid:24033573
  45. 45. Luby J, Belden A, Botteron K, Marrus N, Harms MP, Babb C, et al. The effects of poverty on childhood brain development: The mediating effect of caregiving and stressful life events. JAMA Pediatr. 2013;167(12):1135–42. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.3139. pmid:24165922
  46. 46. Touwen BCL. The preterm in the extrauterine environment. Implications for neurolog. Early Hum Dev. 1980;4(3):287–300. pmid:6774868
  47. 47. Caçola PM, Gabbard C, Montebelo MIL, Santos DCC. The new affordances in the home environment for motor development—infant scale (AHEMD-IS): Versions in English and Portuguese languages. Braz. J. Phys. Ther. 2015;19(6): 507–525. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1590/bjpt-rbf.2014.0112. pmid:26647753
  48. 48. Miquelote AF, Santos DC, Caçola PM, Montebelo MI, Gabbard C. Effect of the home environment on motor and cognitive behavior of infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2012 Jun;35(3):329–34. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.02.002. pmid:22721733
  49. 49. Nascimento AL, Toledo AM, Merey LF, Tudella E, Soares-Marangoni DA. Brief reaching training with “sticky mittens” in preterm infants: Randomized controlled trial. Hum Mov Sci. 2019;63:138–47. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2018.11.015. pmid:30537626
  50. 50. Guimarães EL, Tudella E. Immediate effect of training at the onset of reaching in preterm infants: Randomized clinical trial. J Mot Behav. 2015;47(6):535–49. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2015.1022247. pmid:25893978
  51. 51. Guimarães EL, Cunha AB, Mira DM, Tudella E. Influence of short-term training on the distal adjustments of reaching in preterm infants. J Hum Growth Dev. 2015;25(3):263–70. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.7322/jhgd.89621.
  52. 52. Soares DA, Cunha AB, Tudella E. Differences between late preterm and full-term infants: Comparing effects of a short bout of practice on early reaching behavior. Res Dev Disabil. 2014;35(11):3096–107. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.07.041. pmid:25134076
  53. 53. Bornstein MH, Hahn CS, Suwalsky JTD. Physically Developed and Exploratory Young Infants Contribute to Their Own Long-Term Academic Achievement. Psychol Sci. 2013;24(10):1906–17. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797613479974. pmid:23964000