Figures
Abstract
Sexual selection researchers have traditionally focused on adult sex differences; however, the schedule and pattern of sex-specific ontogeny can provide insights unobtainable from an exclusive focus on adults. Recently, it has been debated whether facial width-to-height ratio (fWHR; bi-zygomatic breadth divided by midface height) is a human secondary sexual characteristic (SSC). Here, we review current evidence, then address this debate using ontogenetic evidence, which has been under-explored in fWHR research. Facial measurements were collected from 3D surface images of males and females aged 3 to 40 (Study 1; US European-descent, n = 2449), and from 2D photographs of males and females aged 7 to 21 (Study 2; Bolivian Tsimane, n = 179), which were used to calculate three fWHR variants (which we call fWHRnasion, fWHRstomion, and fWHRbrow) and two other common facial masculinity ratios (facial width-to-lower-face-height ratio, fWHRlower, and cheekbone prominence). We test whether the observed pattern of facial development exhibits patterns indicative of SSCs, i.e., differential adolescent growth in either male or female facial morphology leading to an adult sex difference. Results showed that only fWHRlower exhibited both adult sex differences as well as the classic pattern of ontogeny for SSCs—greater lower-face growth in male adolescents relative to females. fWHRbrow was significantly wider among both pre- and post-pubertal males in the Bolivian Tsimane sample; post-hoc analyses revealed that the effect was driven by large sex differences in brow height, with females having higher placed brows than males across ages. In both samples, all fWHR measures were inversely associated with age; that is, human facial growth is characterized by greater relative elongation in the mid-face and lower face relative to facial width. This trend continues even into middle adulthood. BMI was also a positive predictor of most of the ratios across ages, with greater BMI associated with wider faces. Researchers collecting data on fWHR should target fWHRlower and fWHRbrow and should control for both age and BMI. Researchers should also compare ratio approaches with multivariate techniques, such as geometric morphometrics, to examine whether the latter have greater utility for understanding the evolution of facial sexual dimorphism.
Citation: Hodges-Simeon CR, Albert G, Richardson GB, McHale TS, Weinberg SM, Gurven M, et al. (2021) Was facial width-to-height ratio subject to sexual selection pressures? A life course approach. PLoS ONE 16(3): e0240284. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240284
Editor: JJ Cray Jr., Ohio State University, UNITED STATES
Received: September 22, 2020; Accepted: January 16, 2021; Published: March 12, 2021
Copyright: © 2021 Hodges-Simeon et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: For the 3D Facial Norms dataset, minimal data available from the FaceBase repository (www.facebase.org; accession number: FB00000491.01). For the Tsimane dataset, minimal data may be found in the supplemental file.
Funding: This work was supported by a Wenner-Gren dissertation fieldwork grant (C.R.H.; www.wennergren.org). The Tsimane Health and Life History project (M.G.) is supported by grants from NSF (BCS-0136761; www.nsf.gov) and NIH/NIA (R01AG024119-01, R56AG024119-06, R01AG024119-07; www.nih.gov). The 3D Norms Facial Database (S.M.W.) was funded by the National Institute of Dental and Craniofacial Research (U01-DE020078, R01-DE016148, and R01-DE027023; www. https://www.nidcr.nih.gov/). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Charles Darwin (1872) [1] used the term secondary sexual characteristic (SSC) to refer to traits that evolve by sexual selection, and which contribute to an individual’s reproductive success through deterring competitors (i.e., intrasexual selection) [2–5] or attracting mates (i.e., intersexual selection) [2, 6]. Sexual selection is the primary explanatory framework for the evolution of sex differences across species, including humans [2, 3, 7–10].
In 2007, Weston et al. [11] proposed a new human SSC—facial width-to-height ratio (fWHR), or the width of the face (between the left and right zygion) divided by the length of the mid-face (from the nasion to the prosthion, referred to as fWHRnasion in the current analyses; see Table 1 and Fig 1 for measurement variants) based on identification of sex differences in a sample of South African crania. Since then, this and similar facial metrics have gained increasing attention in psychology, biological anthropology, and other fields for its persistent association with an array of behavioral, psychosocial, and anatomical traits [12–15]. A number of recent studies, however, highlight inconsistencies in the findings [16–20] and it is now currently debated whether fWHR should be characterized as an SSC [20–24]. We review the current debate, and then argue that important insights may be gained from an ontogenetic approach, which should inform any conclusions drawn from adult populations.
Is fWHR a secondary sexual characteristic (SSC)?
Evolutionary biologists emphasize three joint criteria to assess whether a trait is a product of sexual selection rather than an alternative process (e.g., genetic drift, pleiotropic byproduct) [25].
1. SSCs should be sexually dimorphic, at least during the period(s) of mating competition [2]. Weston et al. [11] first described sex differences in dry bone fWHR among a sample of native southern African crania. However, since then, identification of adult sex differences in fWHR have been inconsistent; several studies have found significant sex differences [11, 13], while others have not [16, 18, 19, 23, 26–28]. A recent meta-analysis of these findings indicated a significant adult sex difference in fWHR, but the magnitude of the effect was small (mean weighted effect size = 0.11) [14]. For comparison, three traits that likely are SSCs—stature, voice pitch, and muscularity—show much larger sex differences, with effect sizes of 1.6 (height, across 53 nations) [29]; 2.4 (vocal fundamental frequency) [30]; and 2.5 (arm muscle volume) [8].
2. SSCs should increase success in mating competition, leading to higher reproductive success (or proxies thereof, such as status, mating success, or judgments of attractiveness) [31–34]. The evidence that men with greater fWHRs have greater reproductive success has been mixed. Studies have shown that men with greater fWHR have greater mating success [35], increased sex drive [36], and more children [37]; whereas other studies have not identified a relationship between men’s fWHR and number of children [19].
Weston et al. [11] originally proposed that a larger fWHR in males (i.e., wider face relative to midface height) may have evolved by intersexual selection (i.e., female choice); however, a meta-analysis showed a significant negative relationship between fWHR and physical attractiveness ratings across 8 studies; that is, women judged men with wider faces to be less attractive [14]. In contrast, there is more compelling support for the notion that fWHR was shaped by intrasexual competition among males. Wider faces seem to be reliably associated with a suite of behavioral traits involved in physical competition (e.g., aggressive behavior in sports) [12, 13] and aggression in both naturalistic and laboratory settings [13, 14, 38, 39]. While several studies found no relationship between fWHR and aggression-linked traits [19], self-reported aggression [18], or behavioral measures of aggression [40], meta-analyses show a strong and consistent relationship between higher male fWHR and perceptions of aggressiveness, fighting ability, masculinity, dominance, and threat by both male and female raters (r = .13-.46 [14, 39, 41]; however, see Dixson et al. [42]). In addition, fWHR is linked to measures of dominance, status, or assertiveness among capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp [43]), macaques (Macaca mulatta [44, 45]), and bonobos (Pan paniscus [46]).
3. SSCs often co-occur with a suite of other behavioral, physiological, and morphological traits that jointly contribute to a particular mating strategy [2]. For instance, selection on larger body size and muscle mass in males (relative to females) usually co-occurs with the behavioral inclination to use these weapons [47, 48], yet fWHR was not associated with grip strength in either sex in a recent study [49]. Other SSCs may not be associated with threat potential directly, but rather function to enhance threat displays when employed (e.g., beards [50–52]; see also Sell et al. [53]) and/or to communicate aggressive intent [54–56]. Evidence suggests this may apply to fWHR; Deska et al. [57] found that the anger expression was more accurately recognized in higher fWHR faces.
Some research suggests fWHR is best understood as a predictor of behavioral strategies that promote status-seeking [58], power, and resource acquisition, such as willingness to cheat or exploit the trust of others to increase financial gain [59–62], risk-taking [63], and narcissism [64] (however, see Kosinski [17]). Many authors reason that the link between these behavioral strategies and fWHR stems from their joint regulation by testosterone [13, 65]. However, amongst adult males, a meta-analysis showed no significant relationship between fWHR and basal testosterone concentrations [65] or androgen receptor gene polymorphisms [66]. For reactive testosterone (i.e., changes in response to challenge), Lefevre et al. [67] found a positive association with fWHR, yet Bird et al. [65] and Kordsmeyer et al. [68] did not. Research on wider face shape and higher prenatal testosterone is promising [69–71], but further studies on hormonal correlates fWHR are needed.
In summary, for each of the three criteria useful in identifying SSCs, the previously published evidence is weak, conflicting, or ambiguous. The first criterion has been under-examined in the literature; that is, the majority of studies focus on adult sex differences. In the present study, we examine the developmental pattern of fWHR (as well as several other facial masculinity ratios) to assess whether these ratios demonstrate sex-specific changes that occur in tandem with the commencement of sexual maturation.
Ontogenetic perspectives on sexual selection
Evolution and ontogeny are closely intertwined because intra- and interspecific evolutionary change in the adult phenotype occurs by means of changing schedules of ontogeny [72–74]. For example, sex differences in adult height can be explained quantitatively by the delayed onset, increased rate, and longer duration of the adolescent growth spurt in males compared with females [75]. This sex-specific pattern of growth suggests that selection for a later and longer growth spurt in males outweighed the costs of later reproduction. Research on fWHR—as well as on sexual selection more generally—has almost exclusively drawn from studies of adult males and females; however, the schedule and pattern of sex-specific development can provide insights on sexual selection pressures unobtainable from studies limited to adults [76–80]. Several types of ontogenetic data should be particularly useful to those interested in sexual selection pressures.
First, SSCs should develop in temporal contiguity with the commencement of mating competition. For some species, this may occur during defined mating seasons [25, 81–84] or transient exposure to potential mates [85, 86], while in others SSC development may canalize during reproductive maturation (i.e., puberty in humans [87–89]). Thus far, only Weston et al. [11] has examined sex differences in fWHR prior to adulthood (see Kesterke et al. [90], Koudelová et al. [91] and Matthews et al. [92] for sex-specific development in non-ratio facial dimensions); therefore, our primary goal is to determine if fWHR (along with several other commonly used facial masculinity ratios) exhibits sex-specific divergence during puberty. To further clarify the developmental pattern and shed light on the role of sexual selection, we assess whether sex differences, if present, arise from male-specific or female-specific growth as a proxy for selection pressures acting on males versus females.
Second, male-specific trait development during or before mating competition is orchestrated by androgens such as testosterone [79, 87–89, 93–95]; thus, an association between testosterone and trait development of masculine features is often treated as evidence for sexual selection in mammalian males [65, 96]. Few studies, however, have examined the association between fWHR and testosterone prior to adulthood. Hodges-Simeon et al. [20] showed that among adolescents, fWHR was not associated with age, and only weakly with testosterone (see also Welker et al. [24] and Hodges-Simeon et al. [22]). This is in stark contrast to more established SSCs (e.g., voice pitch, muscle mass), which show very strong associations with testosterone and age during the adolescent period—a phase when testosterone increases by an order of magnitude in only 5 to 9 years [79, 97, 98].
Third, if fWHR is an SSC, then it should exhibit ontogenetic patterns similar to other human SSCs. SSCs typically emerge together during puberty because they form a functional suite of tactics supporting success in mating competition. Thus, we should see males’ and females’ fWHR diverge in the phase between puberty and adulthood—i.e., adolescence (or potentially in the period between adrenarche and puberty, called juvenility or middle childhood [72, 99–101]). The pattern of development in males may also exhibit a “spurt” (i.e., a period of increasing growth velocity), which is descriptive of the growth pattern of male muscle mass, height, and voice pitch [20]. This pattern is likely due to regulation by testosterone, which itself shows a pronounced spurt [20]. Currently, there is a deficit of findings on the ontogeny of fWHR and other commonly used facial masculinity ratios, which this research seeks to address.
Aims and predictions of the present research
We propose four aims and associated predictions for the present study. Our first goal is to test for the presence or absence of adult sexual dimorphism in fWHR in a large, homogenous (i.e., European-Caucasian; N = 1,477, aged 22–40) sample. Previous studies have diverged, with some showing a significant sex difference (Carré, & McCormick [13], N = 88; Weston et al. [11], N = 121) and others not (Gómez-Valdés et al. [19], N = 4,960; Kramer et al. [26], N = 415; Kramer [27], N = 3,481; Kramer [23], N = 7,941; Ozener [18], N = 470; Robertson, Kingsley, & Ford [28], N = 444), which utilize 2D, 3D, and dry bone skull samples. Kramer et al. [23] has targeted the largest sample of fWHR in dry bone skulls thus far (N = 7,941), showing small but significant sex differences in fWHR in East Asian but not any other populations. We offer the largest sample size to date for fWHR from soft tissue, three-dimensional faces. This is an important complement to the literature on dry bone morphology, as sexual dimorphism may stem not only from divergence in craniofacial growth, but also sex-specific patterns of muscle and fat deposition [8].
Our second aim is to examine sex differences and sex-specific growth in fWHR in sub-adult age groups (i.e., childhood, juvenility, and adolescence), and to determine if sex differences in fWHR are due to male-specific or female-specific growth—questions that have not yet been addressed in the literature. For most human SSCs, pre-pubertal groups show little-to-no difference, while those in later adolescence and adulthood exhibit more observable differences. Sex differences may derive from male-specific growth (i.e., male features growing faster or longer than females’), female-specific growth (i.e., female features growing faster or longer than males’), or a combination of the two. To this end, we measure fWHR among sub-adult males and females in two populations: the large European-Caucasian sample of 3D facial scans (ages 3 to 21) and an indigenous Bolivian Tsimane sample of 2D front-facing photographs (ages 7 to 21).
Our third goal is to examine variation in fWHR growth velocity (i.e., acceleration) across ages as the pattern of ontogeny may yield additional insight. In particular, human male SSCs typically show evidence of a growth spurt during adolescence—rapid acceleration followed by deceleration—due to the influence of testosterone on this trait. This was previously examined in our Tsimane dataset [20], which showed no evidence of a growth spurt in several different fWHR ratios. However, because this sample was small, we address the question again here in our 3D dataset, which offers a larger N.
Our fourth goal is to examine sex differences and sex-specific development in several other commonly used facial masculinity ratios that, unlike fWHR, incorporate mandibular proportions [16, 102, 103]: the ratio of bizygomatic facial width to the width of the face at the mouth (“cheekbone prominence”) and the ratio of bizygomatic width to morphological face height (nasion to bottom of chin; “fWHRlower”, see Fig 1). fWHRlower and cheekbone prominence are smaller in adult men compared to women [16] because of the relatively larger size of the male mandible. In contrast to fWHR, these two facial ratios incorporate the length and breadth of the jaw—an area of the face with a long history of research in biological anthropology [104], clear sexual dimorphism across populations [105, 106], associations with other SSCs [20], and known associations with age and testosterone during development [20, 94, 107]. Further, we include three variants of fWHR used in the literature: fWHRnasion, fWHRbrow, and fWHRstomion (see Fig 1 and Table 1 for a guide to the facial ratios used in the present research and in previous studies). We use this specific terminology here to increase clarity, as each of these variants has separately been termed “fWHR” in the literature. Researchers have largely treated these variants as interchangeable, yet it is unclear whether this decision is justified—i.e., to what extent the variants overlap with one another.
Finally, in all analyses, we control for individual differences in facial adiposity using BMI [108]. Lefevre et al. [16] found sexual dimorphism in fWHR disappeared after controlling for BMI. A meta-analysis of studies before 2015 indicated that higher BMI was associated with larger fWHRs in adults [14], yet only a third of the studies reviewed for this paper control for individual differences in adiposity (see Table 1). This may also be an important control in behavioral research; for example, Deanor et al. [40] identified body weight (which likely overlaps muscle mass), not fWHR, as a predictor of aggression among athletes (see also Mayew [109]).
Methods
3D European/Caucasian sample
Participants.
For the 3D Facial Norms dataset, Institutional ethics (IRB) approval was obtained at each recruitment site and all subjects gave their written informed consent prior to participation (University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board #PRO09060553 and #RB0405013; UT Health Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects #HSC-DB-09-0508; Seattle Children’s Institutional Review Board #12107; University of Iowa Human Subjects Office/Institutional Review Board #200912764 and #200710721). 3D facial scans were obtained from the 3D Facial Norms data set (see Weinberg et al. [110] for a detailed sample description), available at facebase.org (Accession number: FB00000491.01). Participants were recruited from four US cities (Pittsburgh, Seattle, Houston, and Iowa City), primarily through target advertisements. Only individuals who had no history of craniofacial trauma, congenital malformations, or facial surgery were permitted to participate [90].
The sample consisted of 2,449 unrelated individuals of European-Caucasian ancestry between the ages of 3–40 (1502 females and 952 males). Individuals were classified into four age groups: child (3–6 years of age, N = 193), juvenile (7–11 years of age, N = 199), adolescent-to-young adult (12–21 years of age, N = 580), adult (22–40 years of age, N = 1477). We classified ages 19–21 as “adolescents” for several important reasons. First, the end of adolescence is ambiguous and variable across individuals and populations. Western societies arbitrarily set this at 18; however, life history theory marks the end of adolescence with the end of growth and birth of first offspring—events that may vary widely. Second, while male adult height may be reached in the late teens (but not always [72]), growth in other tissues (i.e., muscle mass) often continues after age 18 [111]. Third, endocrine maturation (i.e., rapidly increasing production of sex steroids) usually continues into the early 20s for males [79, 97, 98]. Therefore, development of T-mediated traits will also likely extend past age 18.
Instruments.
Digital stereophotogrammetry was used to obtain 24 landmark distances from the 3D facial scans, from which 5 were used in the present study (nasion, labiale superius, stomion, bottom of the chin, and tragion as a proxy of zygion; see Fig 1). We also utilized two additional distances collected with direct anthropometry using spreading calipers (GPM Switzerland): maximum facial width (zygion to zygion) and mandibular width (gonion to gonion). Previous investigations have verified that data collected from facial images using digital stereophotogrammetry are highly replicable and precise [112]; nevertheless, we examined correlations between fWHR measures calculated using facial width from landmark distances versus direct anthropometry. All were highly correlated:
fWHRnasion (r = .92), fWHRstomion (r = .91), fWHRlower (r = .89), and cheekbone prominence (r = .87). All models described in the results were also run using the caliper-derived ratios, which altered Beta values by only trivial amounts.
Facial landmarks and masculinity ratios.
Facial width was measured from the left to the right tragion, the point marking the notch at the superior margin of the tragus, where the ear cartilage meets the skin of the face. The upper boundary of facial height was measured from the approximate location of the nasion, the midline point where the frontal and nasal bones contact. The lower boundaries for mid-facial height included the labiale superius, the midline point of the vermilion border of the upper lip at the base of the philtrum (for fWHRnasion); the stomion, the midpoint of the labial fissure (fWHRstomion); and the bottom of the chin (fWHRlower). See Fig 1 and Table 1. Ratios were computed by dividing facial width by facial height; greater fWHRs reflect relatively wider faces relative to the height dimensions. Cheekbone prominence was a ratio of facial width to mandibular width. In this sample, mandibular width was measured using a caliper at the left and right gonion. Previous research on cheekbone prominence in front-facing 2D photographs has approximated this location [16] or used the width of the face at the mouth [20, 102]. Information about the location of the brow was not available in the 3D renderings; therefore, of the ratios shown in Fig 1, fWHRbrow could not be used with the 3D sample.
Ratios (rather than measures of individual facial dimensions) are often utilized in previous research for several reasons. First, for 2D photographs in particular, ratios offer greater ease of measurement; that is, no corrections are necessary for distance from the camera, ontogenetic scaling, or deviations from the Frankfurt plane. Second, because of this ease, ratios have been increasingly adopted in disciplines outside of biological anthropology; as such, there is now a growing literature of fWHR results that require evolutionary and ontogenetic explanation.
Anthropometrics.
Self-reported height and weight were collected from each participant, and then used to calculate BMI. See www.facebase.org/facial_norms/notes/ for more information on the sample.
2D Bolivian Tsimane sample
Population.
The Tsimane are a small-scale, kin-based, group of hunter-horticulturalists who reside in the Amazonian lowlands of Bolivia. They obtain relatively few calories from market sources, have little access to modern medicine, and experience high rates of infectious diseases [113–116]. On average, individuals experience high rates of infection; for example, approximately 60% of individuals carry at least one parasite [116]. As such the Tsimane experience high rates of chronic inflammation, characteristic of populations living in environments with high pathogen loads [114].
Participants.
For the Tsimane dataset, Institutional ethics (IRB) approval was obtained by the University of California, Santa Barbara Institutional Review Board. Participants and their parents gave their assent prior to participation. Participants consisted of 139 peripubertal individuals (73 males and 66 females) between the ages of 7 and 21. Participants’ ages were estimated by comparing their self-reported age to their age taken from the Tsimane Health and Life History Project (THLHP) census [113]. When there was a discrepancy between participants’ self-reported and census ages, census age was used (see Hodges-Simeon et al. [87], for further explanation of age estimation methods). Following our 3D sample, participants were divided into juvenile (age 7 to 11) and adolescent (age 12 to 21) age groups.
Facial measurement.
To obtain facial measurements, we first took high-resolution, front-facing color photographs of participants using a 12MP Sony camera. Participants’ heads were positioned along the medial-sagittal plane and they were instructed to have a neutral facial expression. Eleven trained research assistants (RAs), from Boston University and University of California Santa Barbara, placed landmarks on all facial photographs using the image-editing software GIMP and each photograph was processed by three RAs. The research assistants were blind to the hypotheses of the researcher and did not know any of the photographed individuals. The research assistants recorded the x-y coordinates for each landmark of the face twice. The coordinates were averaged (i.e., a total of six x coordinates and six y coordinates per landmark) to establish final landmark coordinates (α = .88, for males, α = .98 for females for the entire sample). Landmarks of interest and ratios are shown in Fig 1. fWHRnasion, fWHRstomion, and fWHRlower were calculated based on the same landmarks as described for the 3D sample above. Because the location of the nasion must be approximated in soft tissue (the nasion is the midline point where the frontal and nasal bones contact), we anticipate more error for this point. fWHRbrow was calculated in the same way as in Carré & McCormick [13]: bi-zygomatic breath was divided by height of the face from the top of the lip to the middle of the brow. Cheekbone prominence was a ratio of facial width to the width of the face at the mouth [20, 102].
Anthropometrics.
Standard anthropometric protocols were used to assess growth and energetic status [117]; participants wore light clothing and no shoes for measurement of height and weight (to determine BMI).
Data screening and analysis.
SPSS 24 was used for all analyses. To correct for small deviations from normality all study variables were log-transformed. Although transformation only altered results by trivial amounts, we report results here using the transformed variables. All assumptions for multivariate analysis (i.e., multi-collinearity, normality, linearity, and homogeneity of variance) were met. Variance Inflation Factors (VIFs) were used to assess multicollinearity; all VIFs < 2.
For analyses, alpha level was set at 0.05 (two-tailed). As a first step, we examined bivariate correlations between all pairs of variables. Point biserial correlations were examined for associations between sex and all other variables of interest (see Table 2). We employed correlations to assess the degree of multicollinearity among different measures of fWHR. Inspection of correlations between different measures of fWHR revealed only small differences across the age groups (i.e., fWHRnasion and fWHRstomion were closely correlated regardless of the age category). Therefore, in the interest of reducing the number of tests, we collapsed across age categories to examine correlations for males and females separately, controlling for age (see Supplement for S1 Table for the 3D sample and S2 Table for the 2D sample). We then proceeded to conduct standard (i.e., simultaneous) multiple regressions, within each face set and age group (Table 3).
Standardized Beta coefficients shown with t statistic in parentheses. Positive values for sex indicate female ratios are larger.
In both samples, males were coded “1” and females were coded “2”; therefore, in the results presented below, positive associations with sex indicate that female means are higher on this trait. Given the importance of accurate coding of sex for the interpretation of results, we examined the association between sex and height—a known SSC—in both samples. In the 3D sample, sex was inversely correlated with body height in adults (r = -.71, p < .001) and in adolescents (r = -.50, p < .001), with adult males showing the expected height advantage over females. Among adolescents in the 2D sample, sex was inversely correlated with height but did not reach conventional levels of significance (r = -.26, p = .08); therefore, we examined the association between sex and voice pitch (data from Hodges-Simeon et al. [87]), which is more strongly dimorphic than height [118]. Sex was positively correlated with voice pitch controlling for age (r = .46, p < .001). That is, being female was associated with higher voice pitch, which confirms accurate sex coding in the 2D sample.
Curve Expert Version 1.5.0 was used to determine a best-fit algorithm for patterns of age-related change in facial masculinity ratios. Goodness-of-fit was assessed using the coefficient of determination (R2). In Hodges-Simeon et al. [20, 87], these methods were used to demonstrate evidence for growth spurts in height and voice pitch.
Results
Correlations
3D European/Caucasian sample.
Point-biserial correlations revealed significant sex differences (positive values indicate females are larger) in fWHRstomion (r = -.08, p = .001), fWHRlower (r = .07, p = .001), cheekbone prominence (r = .08, p = .001), and BMI (r = -.10, p = .001) in adults, but not adolescents, juveniles, and children (see Table 2). Age was correlated with sex in both adults (r = .07, p = .01) and adolescents (r = .15, p < .001), underscoring the need to control for age in further analyses. Collapsing across age groups (and controlling for age), fWHRnasion, fWHRstomion, and fWHRlower showed high collinearity given their shared points of measurement (rs = .78-.96; see S1 Table for exact values). For males and females, cheekbone prominence was moderately associated with fWHRnasion (r = .15 and .07, respectively), fWHRstomion (r = .17 and .09), and fWHRlower (r = .18 and .12). BMI was positively associated with fWHRnasion, fWHRstomion, and fWHRlower for both males and females, indicating increased facial width with increasing BMI. Cheekbone prominence was inversely associated with BMI for females only, indicating that weight gain affects the breadth of the lower face for females. See S1 Table.
2D Bolivian Tsimane sample.
Males had larger fWHRbrow (r = -.44, p = .001, in adolescents; r = -.43, p = .001, in juveniles) and fWHRlower (r = -.29, p = .004, in adolescents), but there were no sex differences in fWHRnasion and cheekbone prominence. See Table 2. We also examined the relationships between fWHR measures to explore the extent to which these measures co-varied. fWHRnasion and fWHRstomion were correlated in males (r = .71, p < .001) and females (r = .40, p < .001), similar to the 3D sample. fWHRbrow was also closely associated with fWHRnasion (r = .82, p < .001 and r = .78, p < .001) and fWHRstomion for males and females, respectively. Cheekbone prominence was significantly associated with fWHRlower (r = .63, p < .001 and r = .31, p < .01). In contrast to the 3D sample, fWHRlower was not significantly associated with fWHRnasion; however, fWHRlower was correlated with fWHRbrow (r = .40, p < .001 and r = .61, p < .001) and fWHRstomion (r = .50, p < .001 and r = .79, p < .001). Also, in contrast to the 3D sample, cheekbone prominence was inversely correlated with fWHRnasion (r = -.40, p < .001 and r = -.39, p < .01) and uncorrelated with fWHRbrow and fWHRstomion. See S2 Table.
Are fWHR and/or other commonly used masculinity ratios sexually dimorphic in adults?
3D European/Caucasian sample.
Zero-order correlations indicated that both BMI and age were associated with sex; therefore, we employed multiple regression to examine the effects of sex on facial masculinity ratios while controlling for these potential confounds. Four separate multiple regression models were employed with sex, age, and BMI as predictors and fWHRnasion, fWHRstomion, fWHRlower, and cheekbone prominence as the outcome variables (see Table 3). Sex was a significant predictor of fWHRstomion (ß = -.05, p < .05), fWHRlower (ß = .09, p < .001), and cheekbone prominence (ß = .08, p < .01), but not fWHRnasion (ß = -.01, p = .84). In other words, males showed the expected pattern of larger mandible breadth (i.e., smaller cheekbone prominence) and longer chin (i.e., smaller fWHRlower). Males showed significantly wider faces relative to the midface, but only when the midface extended to the stomion (i.e., fWHRstomion), and not when it terminated at the labiale superius (fWHRnasion). This finding was surprising given the shared variance in fWHRnasion and fWHRstomion (r = .96; see S1 Table). Post-hoc analyses showed a significant sex difference in upper lip height in this sample (ß = -.38, p < .001) controlling for age and BMI; that is, males have significantly larger upper lip height than females.
BMI was a significant predictor of the outcome variables in all models. Age was also a significant negative predictor for fWHRnasion and fWHRstomion; as individuals age from 22 to 40 years, both of these fWHR measures get smaller, likely reflecting a lengthening of the midface with aging (see Table 3). See also Fig 2 for visual representation of changes in the variables of interest with age.
Are fWHR and/or other commonly used masculinity ratios sexually dimorphic in sub-adults?
3D European/Caucasian sample.
Separate multiple regression models were again conducted for each age group—children, juveniles, and adolescents—and paralleled those for adults. Across all sub-adult age groups, sex was not a significant predictor of any of the masculinity ratios while age was a significant inverse predictor of all facial ratios (see Table 3 for standardized Betas and t statistics). With sub-adult growth, fWHRnasion (ß = -.25, p < .001) and fWHRstomion (ß = -.27, p < .001) became smaller—facial width decreased relative to midface height (i.e., became less masculine based on current conceptualizations of fWHR). fWHRlower (ß = -.32, p < .001) and cheekbone prominence (ß = -.11, p < .05) also became smaller, indicating childhood growth in mandible dimensions relative to bizygomatic width. Similar to the adults, BMI was a significant positive predictor of fWHRnasion, fWHRstomion, and fWHRlower in juvenility and adolescence but not childhood (ßs = .14 - .32; see Table 3). In other words, juveniles/adolescents with greater somatic adiposity (and, by extension, facial adiposity) had wider faces relative to facial height. See Table 3.
2D Bolivian Tsimane sample.
Because brow information was available for the 2D sample but not the 3D sample (see Methods for more information), we examined multiple regression models predicting fWHRbrow as well as the other 4 ratios. In adolescents, sex was a significant negative predictor of fWHRbrow (ß = -.44, p < .001), but not fWHRstomion or fWHRnasion, for which sex approached significance as a positive predictor (ß = .17, p = .09). Again, these results were surprising because fWHRbrow and fWHRnasion were correlated with each other (r = .82, p < .001). Post-hoc analyses were employed to determine if the distance from the nasion to the brow was sexually dimorphic and could be driving the opposing relationships with sex. Controlling for age and BMI, sex was a very strong predictor of nasion-to-brow distance (ß = .72, p < .001), with females having higher-placed brows relative to the nasion position. A similar pattern was found for juveniles (ß = .75, p < .001; see Table 3), indicating this sex difference is present prior to puberty. See Fig 3 for nasion-to-brow distance by age.
Results also showed that sex was a significant positive predictor of fWHRlower in adolescents (ß = .20, p = .04) and approached conventional significance in juveniles (ß = .27, p = .08).
What is the pattern of sex-specific ontogeny for facial masculinity ratios?
3D European/Caucasian sample.
Because analyses thus far showed a significant effect of age on facial ratios across age groups, we explore age-related changes by sex in Fig 2. Visual inspection of results indicates declining facial width relative to height during sub-adult growth as well as during adulthood, supporting conclusions about the effects of age drawn from regressions above.
In order to assess the extent to which facial masculinity ratios exhibit changes in velocity during adolescence—i.e., a growth spurt—we examined whether a sigmoidal model explained more variance than a linear one. Because fWHRstomion, fWHRlower, and cheekbone prominence were found to be sexually dimorphic in adulthood, the pattern of development for each of these ratios was examined for evidence of a growth spurt. As in Hodges-Simeon et al. [20], we found no evidence of changes in facial ratio growth velocity during adolescence.
Visual inspection of the scatterplots suggested that fWHRlower might become sexually dimorphic in later adolescence; therefore, post-hoc analyses were also conducted to determine if restricting the age range to over 14 in both samples changed the results for the adolescent age group. In the 3D sample, fWHRlower was sexually dimorphic (ß = .11, p = .02) among those aged 14 to 21. Restricting the age range did not change the effect of sex for any of the other ratios. In the 2D sample, restricting the age range to 14+ did not substantially change the results; however, fWHRnasion did reach conventional levels of significance (ß = .16, p = .049). That is, over-14 female adolescents had significantly larger fWHRnasions than did males.
Discussion
The goal of the present research was to address ongoing debates on the existence and evolutionary origins of sex-typical variation in fWHR and other facial masculinity ratios using ontogenetic evidence. We examined sex differences in five different ratios across sub-adult and adult age groups in 2D photos and 3D renderings in two distinct populations. Results showed that 3 variables predict significant variation in facial masculinity ratios—sex, age, and BMI. Each reveals potentially important clues to inconsistencies in past fWHR research and suggest agendas for future research.
Summary of results
First, sex was a significant predictor of some but not all facial masculinity ratios. Across both samples, those ratios that incorporated dimensions of the lower face—i.e., the length (fWHRlower) and breadth (cheekbone prominence) of the mandible—suggest a history of sexual selection. In the adult 3D sample (ages 22 to 40), fWHRlower and cheekbone prominence were clearly sexually dimorphic, with males again showing a longer (in terms of fWHRlower where jaw size augments length) and wider (in terms of cheekbone prominence where jaw size augments width) lower face than females. fWHRlower also showed the expected ontogenetic pattern for SSCs; that is, sexual dimorphism developed in the life stage following puberty. In the 2D sample, among adolescents (aged 12 to 21), but not among juveniles (aged 7 to 11), sex was a significant predictor of fWHRlower. In the 3D adolescent sample (aged 12 to 21), sex differences were not found; however, when the age group was restricted to later adolescent ages—i.e., 14 to 21—a significant sex difference emerged, suggesting that lower face development may occur later in adolescence. These findings accord with a long history of research in biological anthropology showing differential growth in the mandible among male Homo sapiens [104], which produces measurable sex differences across diverse populations [92, 105, 106]. These findings also make sense in light of research showing associations between fWHRlower and baseline testosterone levels [20], one testosterone-related genetic variant [119], as well as other testosterone-dependent traits, like upper body strength [20]. Finally, this work accords with research showing that beardedness in male humans may have evolved to amplify the size of the lower face and jaw [42].
Our review of the literature, although not exhaustive, showed substantial variation in the way fWHR is measured when the midface is used as the height dimension (see Table 1). Facial width is relatively consistent across studies; however, midface height has several variants, which we called fWHRnasion, fWHRbrow, and fWHRstomion (see Fig 1). Despite high correlations among these measures, sex differences in these variants were not consistent across measures and samples. In the 3D sample, fWHRstomion was larger in adult males, yet closely correlated fWHRnasion was not dimorphic. Post-hoc analyses showed that this pattern of results was driven by greater upper lip height in males compared with females (also found by Kesterke et al. [90] and Matthews et al. [92]). Sexual dimorphism in upper lip height illustrates that variants of fWHR should not be treated as interchangeable in research. In the 2D sample, fWHRstomion was not dimorphic, while fWHRnasion was significantly larger in females rather than males (among those over 14). It is possible that variation across these samples may be due to inter-population differences in the presence and degree of sexual dimorphism in fWHR; for example, Kramer et al. [23] found significant sex differences in fWHRnasion among East Asian populations but not any other groups. The degree of SSC development may vary with energetic stress [87] and greater sexual dimorphism has been found among energy-abundant societies [120], underscoring the need to sample across a range of diverse human socioecologies, as we have done here.
Our 2D sample included landmarks on the eyebrow, which were not available for the 3D renderings. fWHRbrow was sexually dimorphic, with males showing the expected wider faces relative to females. Again, this was surprising because closely correlated fWHRnasion and fWHRstomion were not dimorphic. Post-hoc analyses revealed that the distance from the nasion to the brow accounts for this pattern of results, with females showing substantially higher brows than males. Like mandible size, this finding accords with previous research on greater supraorbital, or brow ridge, size in male Homo sapiens [121, 122], which is likely associated with lower-set eyebrows. Work in growth modeling has shown that males’ brow ridge grows faster during adolescence, giving rise to observable sex differences by age 16 [92].
Our results also showed that sexual dimorphism in fWHRbrow emerges early, with sex being a significant predictor even in our juvenile sample. The ontogeny of secondary sexual traits is traditionally characterized by differential male and female growth arising from sex steroid hormone increases in puberty [88, 89]. These findings, however, suggest that certain sexually dimorphic face features may diverge prior to puberty—in other periods characterized by hormonal switch points (i.e., prenatal, early post-natal, post-adrenarche). This conclusion is supported by a number of studies that have identified significant early-life sex differences in the face [69–71] and other aspects of the phenotype (e.g., Fouquet et al. [123]). Matthews et al. [92] observed that there were two phases in the emergence of facial sexual dimorphism—ages 5 to 10 (i.e., the post-adrenarche period [101]) and ages 12 onwards. Some aspects of facial sexual dimorphism were present in the first phase and became more exaggerated in the second phase (i.e., forehead, chin, and cheeks), whereas others did not emerge until the second phase (i.e., nose, brow ridge, and upper lip). Sexual dimorphism in several other SSCs begins before puberty; for example, human female infants show greater body fat from birth onwards [124]. The ultimate reasons for different emergence patterns should be addressed in future research; however, one interpretation is that mating and status competition may begin before puberty in humans [99, 100].
A lower brow position may be an important factor in raters’ perceptions of aggressiveness, fighting ability, masculinity, dominance, and threat in those with high fWHRbrow [14, 39, 41]. Research on emotion attribution from facial features has shown that lower-placed eyebrows are perceived as more threatening and aggressive regardless of the facial expression and that raters have greater anger recognition accuracy for high fWHR faces and greater fear accuracy for low fWHR faces (Deska et al. [125], which used brow position). Further, faces where the chin is tilted forward or backward have higher fWHR and are perceived as more intimidating as a result (Hehmen et al. [126], which also used the brow). Lower brow position in males may be a cause or consequence of the evolution of the anger expression and head orientation; that is, sexually dimorphic attributes may have co-evolved with universal facial expressions of anger and fear [127] and may function to enhance threat displays when employed [50–52, 54].
Confounds in fWHR research: Age and BMI
Across both samples, age was a significant inverse predictor of fWHR measures, controlling for sex and BMI. In the 3D sample, age was a consistent negative predictor of facial masculinity ratios from age 3 to adulthood; however, the effect was more pronounced in sub-adult groups. In other words, the face becomes less wide relative to midface height, lower face height, and chin breadth throughout childhood growth, i.e., less “baby-faced” [128]. This is likely a consequence of the decreasing relative size of the cranial vault from birth to adulthood along with increases in nose and mandible growth [92]. In addition, the 3D sample showed that fWHRnasion and fWHRstomion continue to decrease with adult aging, which has been shown in previous research [28, 129], although the slope is not as steep as among sub-adult groups (see Fig 2). This effect may be due to age-related collagen degradation [130] and/or changes in the bony structure [131]. Overall, these findings point to age as an important variable to consider in sample selection and data analysis in fWHR research.
BMI was also a significant predictor of most fWHR measures across juvenile, adolescent, and adult age groups (see Table 3). BMI was used as a proxy measure for fat stores and controlled in all analyses because fat tends to be deposited on the cheeks and chin, increasing facial width. Previous research has consistently shown that BMI is correlated with a higher fWHR [14]; yet a minority of studies reviewed for this paper control for it (see Table 1). The role of BMI in predicting individual differences in facial masculinity ratios speaks to the importance of examining fWHR in both dry bone and soft tissue faces. Evidence suggests that there may be differential selection on bone and fat/muscle in humans and that each may separately contribute to increases in fWHR. For example, in one forensic sample, men with lower fWHRs were significantly more likely to die from contact violence than were men with higher fWHR, suggesting that men with relatively wider faces were more likely to survive aggressive encounters with other men [132]. The authors hypothesized that greater zygomatic buttressing may have benefited ancestral men by reducing the negative effects of craniofacial impact. Yet measures of fWHR from 2D photographs cannot distinguish facial breadth due to bony dimensions, which are more substantial in men, versus fat deposits, which tend to be greater in women [8]. Previous studies have shown that the cheek region is sexually dimorphic [92] and our results showed that BMI affects cheekbone prominence in females but not males. Finally, little research has considered how sex differences in facial muscle may impact fWHR dimensions; one recent study showed that the brachyfacial face type, which overlaps with high fWHR, has greater masseter volume than more narrow face types [133].
Ontogeny and sexual selection
The broader goal of this research was to emphasize the importance of using ontogenetic data to address questions in sexual selection research, using fWHR as a model case. We point to four questions that may be asked of this type of data that should corroborate conclusions drawn from data on adults, providing a roadmap for future researchers to use developmental patterns to substantiate claims about sexual selection pressures. First, do sex differences arise in coordination with the onset of mate competition? Second, do sex differences arise from differential male or female growth? Third, does the purported sexually selected trait exhibit a spurt? And finally, do these traits co-vary with sex steroid hormones and/or other SSCs? Our results show that only fWHRlower exhibits the expected pattern of ontogeny for a sexually selected male trait.
As a further example of an SSC with a clearer history of sexual selection, we point to research on the low human male voice. During puberty, increased production of testosterone causes males’ vocal folds to thicken and their larynxes to descend, producing a lower pitched and more resonant sounding voice [97, 134, 135]. Male adolescents experience a decrease in fundamental and formant frequencies, which jointly contribute to perceived lower pitch, as their vocal folds thicken and lengthen. This decrease happens in a “spurt” [87]. By adulthood, the sex difference in fundamental frequency is over 5 standard deviations [118], and may be associated with variation in testosterone ([135]; however, see Arnocky et al. [136] and Landry et al., [137]). Lower pitched voices are rated as more attractive-sounding by women and more dominant-sounding by both sexes [138, 139]. Furthermore, in one natural-fertility population, men with lower pitched voices were found to father more offspring [31]. Finally, sexually dimorphic vocal parameters are associated with body size [140], muscle mass during adolescence [80], self-report aggressiveness [118], and perceptions of aggressive intent [55, 56]. These various sources of evidence jointly lend greater confidence to the assertion that male vocal traits are SSCs. Most measures of fWHR do not meet this evidentiary standard.
Limitations
This research has several limitations. We sought to compare the pattern of fWHR ontogeny in two distinct populations (European-decent Caucasians and indigenous-decent Bolivians); however, there were methodological differences between the two that prohibit a direct comparison. First, besides being 3D and 2D respectively, landmarks were placed by a different set of researchers, which could have introduced bias. Further, cheekbone prominence was measured using a caliper distance in the 3D sample and a landmark distance in the 2D sample, based on what was available in the datasets. Further research is needed which directly compares across populations using the same methodology (see Kramer [23]). Second, the nasion landmark was used in Weston et al.’s [11] original research on facial width in dry bone samples; however, it should be used with caution in soft tissue studies. The nasion refers to the midline point where the frontal and nasal bones contact (i.e., the nasofrontal suture). Although informed by previous research [141], this exact position poses more of a challenge in soft tissue photos or renderings; therefore, there may be a larger degree of error in this landmark. Our results suggest that when fWHR is measured in soft tissue, brow position should be used rather than the nasion. Finally, this research highlights the importance of age, yet the data are cross-sectional. Future studies on intra-individual longitudinal change would help clarify the effect of age and BMI on sex differences in fWHR.
An additional limitation of all fWHR research is the drawbacks of using a simple ratio or discrete dimensions to describe complex, multidimensional features such as face shape. Faces are composite phenotypes that vary in a number of interrelated dimensions; therefore, changes in any single dimensions may push or pull other aspects of the face in ways that are not reflected by a simple ratio. For instance, Costa et al. [142] manipulated fWHR, yet in order to keep head size constant, low fWHR faces had longer chins and smaller relative eye size (see Fig 1, pg. 3). Further, higher fWHR seems to be consistently rated as more masculine in males, but not in females (See Geniole et al. [14], Table 1). This is particularly unusual as most human male SSCs (e.g., higher muscle mass, broader shoulders, lower voice, larger size) shape impressions of masculinity and physical dominance in both males and females [143–145]. This puzzling pattern of results may be rooted in the association between fWHR and other dimorphic features not fully captured in the ratio. In other words, it is difficult to know if the association between fWHR and a wide variety of tested variables lie in the ratio itself or closely correlated features. Therefore, true experimental manipulation of fWHR is not possible and researchers interested in facial sexual dimorphism should consider multivariate approaches [146], such as geometric morphometrics [69, 90–92].
Conclusions
These findings add an ontogenetic perspective to the ongoing debate on the history of sexual selection on fWHR. Our results show that only fWHRlower exhibits the classic pattern of ontogeny for a sexually selected human male trait—i.e., adult sex differences in fWHRlower along with greater lower-face growth in males relative to females during adolescence. These findings also highlight potential confounds that may be responsible for inconsistent findings in the fWHR literature (i.e., age—due to both sub-adult growth and adult ageing—and BMI), and also reveal via post-hoc analysis some features (brow position and lip height) that deserve further study as possible targets of sexual selection.
Supporting information
S1 Table. Multicollinearity in fWHR measures [partial correlations for females (upper triangle) and males (lower triangle), controlling for age (3D sample)].
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240284.s002
(DOCX)
S2 Table. Multicollinearity in fWHR measures [partial correlations for females (upper triangle) and males (lower triangle), controlling for age (2D sample)].
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240284.s003
(DOCX)
Acknowledgments
The authors extend many thanks to the Tsimane participants and their families, as well as the Tsimane Health and Life History researchers and staff.
References
- 1.
Darwin C. (1871). The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex (1st ed.). London: John Murray. ISBN 978-0-8014-2085-6.
- 2.
Andersson M. (1994). Sexual selection (Vol. 72). Princeton University Press.
- 3. Puts D. A. (2010). Beauty and the beast: Mechanisms of sexual selection in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(3), 157–175.
- 4. Buss D. M. (1988). The evolution of human intrasexual competition: tactics of mate attraction. Journal of personality and social psychology, 54(4), 616–628. pmid:3367282
- 5.
Lindenfors P., & Tullberg B. S. (2011). Evolutionary aspects of aggression: the importance of sexual selection. In Advances in genetics (Vol. 75, pp. 7–22). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-380858-5.00009-5 pmid:22078475
- 6. Buss D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and brain sciences, 12(1), 1–14.
- 7. Conroy-Beam D., Buss D. M., Pham M. N., & Shackelford T. K. (2015). How sexually dimorphic are human mate preferences? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(8), 1082–1093. pmid:26068718
- 8. Lassek W. D., & Gaulin S. J. (2009). Costs and benefits of fat-free muscle mass in men: Relationship to mating success, dietary requirements, and native immunity. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30(5), 322–328.
- 9. Penton-Voak I. S., Perrett D. I., Castles D. L., Kobayashi T., Burt D. M., Murray L. K., et al. (1999). Menstrual cycle alters face preference. Nature, 399(6738), 741–742. pmid:10391238
- 10. Plavcan J. M. (2012). Sexual size dimorphism, canine dimorphism, and male-male competition in primates. Human Nature, 23(1), 45–67. pmid:22388772
- 11. Weston E. M., Friday A. E., & Liò P. (2007). Biometric evidence that sexual selection has shaped the Hominin face. PLoS one, 2(8). pmid:17684556
- 12. Carré J. M., McCormick C. M., & Mondloch C. J. (2009). Facial structure is a reliable cue of aggressive behavior. Psychological Science, 20(10), 1194–1198. pmid:19686297
- 13. Carré J. M., & McCormick C. M. (2008). In your face: Facial metrics predict aggressive behaviour in the laboratory and in varsity and professional hockey players. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275(1651), 2651–2656. pmid:18713717
- 14. Geniole S. N., Denson T. F., Dixson B. J., Carré J. M., & Mccormick C. M. (2015). Evidence from meta-analyses of the facial width-to-height ratio as an evolved cue of threat. PLOS ONE, 10(7). pmid:26181579
- 15. Haselhuhn M. P., Ormiston M. E., & Wong E. M. (2015). Men’s facial width-to-height ratio predicts aggression: A meta-analysis. PLoS ONE, 8, 10(4). pmid:25849992
- 16. Lefevre C. E., Lewis G. J., Bates T. C., Dzhelyova M., Coetzee V., Deary I. J., et al. (2012). No evidence for sexual dimorphism of facial width-to-height ratio in four large adult samples. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(6), 623–627.
- 17. Kosinski M. (2017). Facial width-to-height ratio does not predict self-reported behavioral tendencies. Psychological Science, 28(11), 1675–1682. pmid:28976810
- 18. Özener B. (2012). Facial width-to-height ratio in a Turkish population is not sexually dimorphic and is unrelated to aggressive behavior. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(3), 169–173.
- 19. Gómez-Valdés J., Hünemeier T., Quinto-Sánchez M., Paschetta C., Azevedo S. D., González M. F., et al. (2013). Lack of support for the association between facial shape and aggression: A reappraisal based on a worldwide population genetics perspective. PLoS ONE, 8(1). pmid:23326328
- 20. Hodges-Simeon C. R., Sobraske K. N. H., Samore T., Gurven M., & Gaulin S. J. (2016). Facial width-to-height ratio (fWHR) is not associated with adolescent testosterone levels. PloS one, 11(4), e0153083. pmid:27078636
- 21. Dixson B. J. (2018). Is male facial width-to-height ratio the target of sexual selection? Archives of sexual behavior, 47(4), 827–828. pmid:29508170
- 22. Hodges-Simeon C.R., Richardson G., Hanson-Sobraske K., Samore T., Gurven M., & Gaulin S.J.C. Response: Commentary: fWHR is not associated with pubertal testosterone. Frontiers in Psychology—Evolutionary Psychology, 19. pmid:29520245
- 23. Kramer R. S. (2017). Sexual dimorphism of facial width-to-height ratio in human skulls and faces: A meta-analytical approach. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(3), 414–420.
- 24. Welker K. M., Bird B. M., & Arnocky S. (2016). Commentary: Facial Width-to-Height Ratio (fWHR) Is Not Associated with Adolescent Testosterone Levels. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. pmid:27917130
- 25. Järvi T., Røskaft E., Bakken M., & Zumsteg B. (1987). Evolution of variation in male secondary sexual characteristics. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 20(3), 161–169.
- 26. Kramer R. S., Jones A. L., & Ward R. (2012). A lack of sexual dimorphism in width-to-height ratio in white European faces using 2D photographs, 3D scans, and anthropometry. PloS one, 7(8), e42705. pmid:22880088
- 27. Kramer R. S. (2015). Facial width-to-height ratio in a large sample of commonwealth games athletes. Evolutionary Psychology, 13(1), 147470491501300112.
- 28. Robertson J. M., Kingsley B. E., & Ford G. C. (2017). Sexually dimorphic faciometrics in humans from early adulthood to late middle age: Dynamic, declining, and differentiated. Evolutionary Psychology, 15(3), 1474704917730640. pmid:28901197
- 29. Lippa R. A. (2009). Sex differences in sex drive, sociosexuality, and height across 53 nations: Testing evolutionary and social structural theories. Archives of Sexual Beavhior, 38, 631–651.
- 30. Vogel A. P., Maruff P., Snyder P. J., & Mundt J. C. (2009). Standardization of pitch-range settings in voice acoustic analysis. Behavior Research Methods, 41(2), 318–324. pmid:19363172
- 31. Apicella C. L., Feinberg D. R., & Marlowe F. W. (2007). Voice pitch predicts reproductive success in male hunter-gatherers. Biology letters, 3(6), 682–684. pmid:17895219
- 32. Hughes S. M., Dispenza F., & Gallup G. G. Jr (2004). Ratings of voice attractiveness predict sexual behavior and body configuration. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(5), 295–304.
- 33. Gontard-Danek M. C., & Møller A. P. (1999). The strength of sexual selection: a meta-analysis of bird studies. Behavioral ecology, 10(5), 476–486.
- 34. Grueter C. C, Isler K., & Dixson B. J. 2015. Are primate badges of status adaptive in large groups? Evolution and Human Behavior, 36, 398–406.
- 35. Valentine K. A., Li N. P., Penke L., & Perrett D. I. (2014). Judging a man by the width of his face: The role of facial ratios and dominance in mate choice at speed-dating events. Psychological Science, 25(3), 806–811. pmid:24458269
- 36. Arnocky S., Carré J. M., Bird B. M., Moreau B. J., Vaillancourt T., Ortiz T., et al. (2018a). The facial width-to-height ratio predicts sex drive, sociosexuality, and intended infidelity. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 47(5), 1375–1385. pmid:28929303
- 37. Loehr J., & O’Hara R. B. (2013). Facial morphology predicts male fitness and rank but not survival in Second World War Finnish soldiers. Biology letters, 9(4), 20130049. pmid:23658003
- 38. Welker K. M., Goetz S. M., Galicia S., Liphardt J., & Carré J. M. (2014). An examination of the associations between facial structure, aggressive behavior, and performance in the 2010 World Cup Association football players. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 1(1), 17–29.
- 39. Zilioli S., Sell A. N., Stirrat M., Jagore J., Vickerman W., & Watson N. V. (2015). Face of a fighter: Bizygomatic width as a cue of formidability. Aggressive Behavior, 41(4), 322–330. pmid:24910133
- 40. Deaner R. O., Goetz S. M., Shattuck K., & Schnotala T. (2012). Body weight, not facial width-to-height ratio, predicts aggression in pro hockey players. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(2), 235–238.
- 41. Geniole S., & McCormick C. (2015). Facing our ancestors: Judgements of aggression are consistent and related to the facial width-to-height ratio in men irrespective of beards. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(4), 279–285.
- 42. Dixson B. J. W, Lee A. J., Sherlock J. M., & Talamas S. N. (2017). Beneath the beard: Do facial morphometrics influence the strength of judgments of men’s beardedness? Evolution and Human Behavior. 38, 164–174.
- 43. Lefevre C. E., Wilson V. A., Morton F. B., Brosnan S. F., Paukner A., & Bates T. C. (2014). Facial width-to-height ratio relates to alpha status and assertive personality in capuchin monkeys. PloS one, 9(4), e93369. pmid:24705247
- 44. Altschul D. M., Robinson L. M., Coleman K., Capitanio J. P., & Wilson V. A. D. (2019). An Exploration of the Relationships Among Facial Dimensions, Age, Sex, Dominance Status, and Personality in Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta). International Journal of Primatology, 1–21. pmid:31593962
- 45. Borgi M., & Majolo B. (2016). Facial width-to-height ratio relates to dominance style in the genus Macaca. PeerJ, 4, e1775. pmid:27019780
- 46. Martin J. S., Staes N., Weiss A., Stevens J. M. G., & Jaeggi A. V. (2019). Facial width-to-height ratio is associated with agonistic and affiliative dominance in bonobos (Pan paniscus). Biology Letters, 15(8), 20190232. pmid:31455170
- 47. Sell A., Cosmides L., Tooby J., Sznycer D., Rueden C. V., & Gurven M. (2009). Human adaptations for the visual assessment of strength and fighting ability from the body and face. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1656), 575–584. pmid:18945661
- 48. Sell A., Sznycer D., Cosmides L., Tooby J., Krauss A., Nisu S., et al. (2017). Physically strong men are more militant: A test across four countries. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(3), 334–340.
- 49. MacDonell E., Geniole S., & McCormick C. (2018). Force versus fury: Sex differences in the relationships among physical and psychological threat potential, the facial width-to-height ratio, and judgements of aggressiveness. Aggressive Behavior, 44(5), 512–523.
- 50. Craig B. M., Nelson N. L., & Dixson B. J. W. 2019. Sexual selection, agonistic signalling, and the effect of beards on men’s anger displays. Psychological Science, 30, 728–738. pmid:30908116
- 51. Dixson A. F., Dixson B. J., & Anderson M. J. 2005. Sexual selection and the evolution of visually conspicuous sexually dimorphic traits in male monkeys, apes, and human beings. Annual Review of Sex Research, 16, 1–17. pmid:16913285
- 52. Dixson B. J., & Vasey P. L. (2012). Beards augment perceptions of men’s age, social status, and aggressiveness, but not attractiveness. Behavioral Ecology, 23(3), 481–490.
- 53. Sell A., Cosmides L., & Tooby J. (2014). The human anger face evolved to enhance cues of strength. Evol. Hum. Behav. 35, 425–429.
- 54. Zhang J., & Reid S. A. (2017). Aggression in young men high in threat potential increases after hearing low-pitched male voices: two tests of the retaliation-cost model. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(4), 513–521.
- 55. Zhang J., Chen B.B., Hodges-Simeon C.R., Reid S.A., Albert G.*, & Gaulin S.J.C. (2020). Elevated recognition accuracy for low-pitched male voices in men with higher threat potential: Further evidence for the retaliation-cost model in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior. pmid:33100820
- 56. Zhang J., Hodges-Simeon C.R., Reid S.A., & Gaulin S.J.C. (2020). Pitch-lowering enhances men’s perceived aggressive intent independent of fighting ability. Evolution and Human Behavior. pmid:33100820
- 57. Deska J. C., Lloyd E. P., & Hugenberg K. (2018b). The face of fear and anger: Facial width-to-height ratio biases recognition of angry and fearful expressions. Emotion, 18(3), 453.
- 58. Lewis G. J., Lefevre C. E., & Bates T. C. (2012). Facial width-to-height ratio predicts achievement drive in US presidents. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(7), 855–857
- 59. Geniole S. N., Keyes A. E., Carré J. M., & McCormick C. M. (2014a). Fearless dominance mediates the relationship between the facial width-to-height ratio and willingness to cheat. Personality and Individual Differences, 57, 59–64.
- 60. Haselhuhn M. P., & Wong E. M. (2011). Bad to the bone: facial structure predicts unethical behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1728), 571–576. pmid:21733897
- 61. Jia Y., Lent L. V., & Zeng Y. (2014). Masculinity, testosterone, and financial misreporting. Journal of Accounting Research, 52(5), 1195–1246.
- 62. Stirrat M., & Perrett D. I. (2010). Valid facial cues to cooperation and trust: Male facial width and trustworthiness. Psychological science, 21(3), 349–354. pmid:20424067
- 63. Welker K. M., Goetz S. M., & Carré J. M. (2015). Perceived and experimentally manipulated status moderates the relationship between facial structure and risk-taking. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(6), 423–429.
- 64. Noser E., Schoch J., & Ehlert U. (2018). The influence of income and testosterone on the validity of facial width-to-height ratio as a biomarker for dominance. PloS one, 13(11), e0207333. pmid:30412629
- 65. Bird B. M., Jofré V. S. C., Geniole S. N., Welker K. M., Zilioli S., Maestripieri D., et al. (2016). Does the facial width-to-height ratio map onto variability in men’s testosterone concentrations? Evolution and Human Behavior, 37(5), 392–398.
- 66. Eisenbruch A. B., Lukaszewski A. W., Simmons Z. L., Arai S., & Roney J. R. (2018). Why the wide face? Androgen receptor gene polymorphism does not predict men’s facial width-to-height ratio. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 4(2), 138–151.
- 67. Lefevre C. E., Lewis G. J., Perrett D. I., & Penke L. (2013). Telling facial metrics: facial width is associated with testosterone levels in men. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(4), 273–279.
- 68. Kordsmeyer T. L., Freund D., Pita S. R., Jünger J., & Penke L. (2019). Further Evidence that Facial Width-to-Height Ratio and Global Facial Masculinity Are Not Positively Associated with Testosterone Levels. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 5(2), 117–130.
- 69. Bulygina E., Mitteroecker P., & Aiello L. (2006). Ontogeny of facial dimorphism and patterns of individual development within one human population. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 131(3), 432–443. pmid:16596605
- 70. Weinberg S. M., Parsons T. E., Raffensperger Z. D., & Marazita M. L. (2015). Prenatal sex hormones, digit ratio, and face shape in adult males. Orthodontics & craniofacial research, 18(1), 21–26.
- 71. Whitehouse A. J. O., Gilani S. Z., Shafait F., Mian A., Tan D. W., Maybery M. T., et al. (2015). Prenatal testosterone exposure is related to sexually dimorphic facial morphology in adulthood. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. pmid:26400740
- 72.
Bogin B. (1999). Patterns of human growth (Vol. 88). Cambridge University Press.
- 73. Gould S. J. (1977). Ontogeny and phylogeny. Harvard University Press.
- 74. Leigh S. R., & Shea B. T. (1995). Ontogeny and the evolution of adult body size dimorphism in apes. American Journal of Primatology, 36(1), 37–60. pmid:31924084
- 75.
Hauspie R. & Roelants M. (2012). Adolescent growth. In Cameron N., Bogin B. (eds). Human growth and development (pp. 1–22). Academic Press.
- 76. Badyaev A.V. (2002). Growing apart: an ontogenetic perspective on the evolution of sexual size dimorphism. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 17(8), 369–378.
- 77. Mank J.E., Nam K., Brunstrom B., Ellegren H. (2010). Ontogenetic Complexity of Sexual Dimorphism and Sex-Specific Selection. Mol. Biol. Evol. 27(7):1570–1578. pmid:20142440
- 78. Taylor A.B. (1997). Relative Growth, Ontogeny, and Sexual Dimorphism in Gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla and G. g. beringei): Evolutionary and Ecological Considerations. American Journal of Primatology, 43, 1–31. pmid:9294638
- 79. Hodges-Simeon C. R., Gurven M., & Gaulin S. J. (2015). The low male voice is a costly signal of phenotypic quality among Bolivian adolescents. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(4), 294–302.
- 80. Hodges-Simeon C. R., Gurven M., Puts D. A., & Gaulin S. J. (2014). Vocal fundamental and formant frequencies are honest signals of threat potential in peripubertal males. Behavioral Ecology, 25(4), 984–988. pmid:25024638
- 81. Burger D. K., Saucier J. M., Iwaniak A. N., & Saucier D. M. (2013). Seasonal and sex differences in the hippocampus of a wild rodent. Behavioural Brain Research, 236, 131–138. pmid:22974551
- 82. Galea L. A. M., Kavaliers M., Ossenkopp K-P., Innes D., & Hargreaves E. L. (1994). Sexually dimorphic spatial learning varies seasonally in two populations of deer mice. Brain Research, 635, 18–26. pmid:8173954
- 83. Pyter L.M., Reader B. F., & Nelson R. J. (2005). Short photoperiods impair spatial learning and alter hippocampal dendritic morphology in adult male white-footed mice (Peromyscus leucopus). The Journal of Neuroscience 25(18), 4521–4526. pmid:15872099
- 84. Smith GT, Brenowitz EA, Wingfield JC (1997) Seasonal changes in the size of the avian song control nucleus HVC defined by multiple histological markers. J Comp Neurol 381:253–261. pmid:9133567
- 85. Amstislavskaya T. G., & Popova N. K. (2004). Female-induced sexual arousal in male mice and rats: behavioral and testosterone response. Hormones and Behavior, 46(5), 544–550. pmid:15555495
- 86. Roney J. R., Lukaszewski A. W., & Simmons Z. L. (2007). Rapid endocrine responses of young men to social interactions with young women. Hormones and Behavior, 52(3), 326–333. pmid:17585911
- 87. Hodges-Simeon C. R., Gurven M., Cárdenas R. A., & Gaulin S. J. (2013). Voice change as a new measure of male pubertal timing: A study among Bolivian adolescents. Annals of Human Biology, 40(3), 209–219. pmid:23388046
- 88.
Ellison P. T. (2009). On fertile ground: A natural history of human reproduction. Harvard University Press.
- 89.
Hochberg Z. E. (2011). Evo-devo of child growth: treatise on child growth and human evolution. John Wiley & Sons.
- 90. Kesterke M. J., Raffensperger Z. D., Heike C. L., Cunningham M. L., Hecht J. T., Kau C. H., et al. (2016). Using the 3D Facial Norms Database to investigate craniofacial sexual dimorphism in healthy children, adolescents, and adults. Biology of sex differences, 7(1), 1–14. pmid:27110347
- 91. Koudelová J., Hoffmannová E., Dupej J., & Velemínská J. (2019). Simulation of facial growth based on longitudinal data: age progression and age regression between 7 and 17 years of age using 3D surface data. PloS one, 14(2), e0212618. pmid:30794623
- 92. Matthews H. S., Penington A. J., Hardiman R., Fan Y., Clement J. G., Kilpatrick N. M., et al. (2018). Modelling 3D craniofacial growth trajectories for population comparison and classification illustrated using sex-differences. Scientific reports, 8(1), 1–11. pmid:29311619
- 93. Marečková K., Weinbrand Z., Chakravarty M. M., Lawrence C., Aleong R., Leonard G., et al. (2011). Testosterone-mediated sex differences in the face shape during adolescence: subjective impressions and objective features. Hormones and Behavior, 60(5), 681–690. pmid:21983236
- 94. Verdonck A. (1999). Effect of low-dose testosterone treatment on craniofacial growth in boys with delayed puberty. The European Journal of Orthodontics, 21(2), 137–143. pmid:10327737
- 95. Pyter L. M., Trainor B. C., & Nelson R. J. (2006). Testosterone and photoperiod interact to affect spatial learning and memory in adult male white-footed mice (Peromyscus leucopus). European Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 3056–3052.
- 96. Folstad I., & Karter A. J. (1992). Parasites, bright males, and the immunocompetence handicap. The American Naturalist, 139(3), 603–622.
- 97. Butler G. E., Walker R. F., Walker R. V., Teague P., Riad-Fahmy D., & Ratcliffe S. G. (1989). Salivary testosterone levels and the progress of puberty in the normal boy. Clinical Endocrinology, 30(5), 587–596. pmid:2605791
- 98. Elmlinger M. W., Kühnel W., Wormstall H., & Döller P. C. (2005). Reference intervals for testosterone, androstenedione and SHBG levels in healthy females and males from birth until old age. Clinical laboratory, 51(11–12), 625–632. pmid:16329620
- 99. McHale T. S., Gray P. B., Hodges-Simeon C. R., Zava D. T., Albert G., Chan K. C., et al. (2020a). Juvenile Children’s Salivary Aldosterone and Cortisone Decrease during Informal Math and Table-Tennis Competitions. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 1–23.
- 100. McHale T. S., Chee W. C., Hodges-Simeon C. R., Zava D. T., Albert G., Chan K. C., et al. (2020b). Salivary aldosterone and cortisone respond differently to high-and low-psychologically stressful soccer competitions. Journal of Sports Sciences, 1–10.
- 101. Campbell B. (2011). Adrenarche in comparative perspective. American Journal of Human Biology, 23(1), 44–52. pmid:21140467
- 102. Penton-Voak I. S., Jones B. C., Little A. C., Baker S., Tiddeman B., Burt D. M., et al. (2001). Symmetry, sexual dimorphism in facial proportions and male facial attractiveness. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 268(1476), 1617–1623. pmid:11487409
- 103. Little A. C., Jones B. C., Waitt C., Tiddeman B. P., Feinberg D. R., Perrett D. I., et al. (2008). Symmetry is related to sexual dimorphism in faces: data across culture and species. PloS one, 3(5), e2106. pmid:18461131
- 104. Lundström A., & Lysell L. (1953). An anthropological examination of a group of medieval Danish skulls, with particular regard to the jaws and occlusal conditions. Acta Odontologica Scandinavica, 11(2), 111–128. pmid:13123857
- 105. Franklin D., O’Higgins P., & Oxnard C. E. (2008). Sexual dimorphism in the mandible of indigenous South Africans: A geometric morphometric approach. South African Journal of Science, 104(3–4), 101–106.
- 106. Saini V., Srivastava R., Rai R. K., Shamal S. N., Singh T. B., & Tripathi S. K. (2011). Mandibular ramus: An indicator for sex in fragmentary mandible. Journal of forensic sciences, 56, S13–S16. pmid:20950321
- 107. Snodell S. F., Nanda R. S., & Currier G. F. (1993). A longitudinal cephalometric study of transverse and vertical craniofacial growth. American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics, 104(5), 471–483. pmid:8237899
- 108. Coetzee V., Chen J., Perrett D. I., & Stephen I. D. (2010). Deciphering faces: Quantifiable visual cues to weight. Perception, 39(1), 51–61. pmid:20301846
- 109. Mayew W. J. (2013). Reassessing the association between facial structure and baseball performance: a comment on Tsujimura & Banissy (2013). Biology Letters, 9(5), 20130538. pmid:24026348
- 110. Weinberg S. M., Raffensperger Z. D., Kesterke M. J., Heike C. L., Cunningham M. L., Hecht J. T., et al. (2016). The 3D Facial Norms Database: Part 1. A web-based craniofacial anthropometric and image repository for the clinical and research community. The Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 53(6), e185–e197. pmid:26492185
- 111. Schutz Y., Kyle U. U. G., & Pichard C. (2002). Fat-free mass index and fat mass index percentiles in Caucasians aged 18–98 y. International journal of obesity, 26(7), 953–960. pmid:12080449
- 112. Aldridge K., Boyadjiev S. A., Capone G. T., DeLeon V. B., & Richtsmeier J. T. (2005). Precision and error of three-dimensional phenotypic measures acquired from 3dMD photogrammetric images. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A, 138(3), 247–253. pmid:16158436
- 113. Gurven M., Kaplan H., & Supa A. Z. (2007). Mortality experience of Tsimane Amerindians of Bolivia: regional variation and temporal trends. American Journal of Human Biology, 19(3), 376–398. pmid:17421012
- 114. Gurven M., Kaplan H., Winking J., Finch C., & Crimmins E. M. (2008). Aging and inflammation in two epidemiological worlds. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 63(2), 196–199. pmid:18314457
- 115. Martin M. A., Lassek W. D., Gaulin S. J., Evans R. W., Woo J. G., Geraghty S. R., et al. (2012). Fatty acid composition in the mature milk of Bolivian forager‐horticulturalists: controlled comparisons with a US sample. Maternal & Child Nutrition, 8(3), 404–418. pmid:22624983
- 116. Vasunilashorn S., Crimmins E. M., Kim J. K., Winking J., Gurven M., Kaplan H., et al. (2010). Blood lipids, infection, and inflammatory markers in the Tsimane of Bolivia. American Journal of Human Biology, 22(6), 731–740. pmid:20721985
- 117.
Lohman T. G., Roche A. F., & Martorell R. (1988). Anthropometric standardization reference manual (Vol. 177, pp. 3–8). Champaign: Human kinetics books.
- 118. Puts D. A., Apicella C. L., & Cárdenas R. A. (2011). Masculine voices signal men’s threat potential in forager and industrial societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1728), 601–609. pmid:21752821
- 119. Roosenboom J., Indencleef K., Lee M. K., Hoskens H., White J. D., Liu D., et al. (2018). SNPs associated with testosterone levels influence human facial morphology. Frontiers in genetics, 9, 497. pmid:30405702
- 120. German A., & Hochberg Z. E. (2020). Sexual dimorphism of size ontogeny and life history. Frontiers in Pediatrics, 8. pmid:32793524
- 121. Claes P., Walters M., Shriver M. D., Puts D., Gibson G., Clement J., et al. (2012). Sexual dimorphism in multiple aspects of 3D facial symmetry and asymmetry defined by spatially dense geometric morphometrics. Journal of anatomy, 221(2), 97–114. pmid:22702244
- 122. Garvin H. M., & Ruff C. B. (2012). Sexual dimorphism in skeletal browridge and chin morphologies determined using a new quantitative method. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 147(4), 661–670. pmid:22371071
- 123. Fouquet M., Pisanski K., Mathevon N., & Reby D. (2016). Seven and up: individual differences in male voice fundamental frequency emerge before puberty and remain stable throughout adulthood. Royal Society open science, 3(10), 160395. pmid:27853555
- 124. Koo W. W., Walters J. C., & Hockman E. M. (2000). Body composition in human infants at birth and postnatally. The Journal of nutrition, 130(9), 2188–2194. pmid:10958811
- 125. Deska J. C., Lloyd E. P., & Hugenberg K. (2018a). Facing humanness: Facial width-to-height ratio predicts ascriptions of humanity. Journal of personality and social psychology, 114(1), 75. pmid:28846003
- 126. Hehman E., Leitner J. B., & Gaertner S. L. (2013). Enhancing static facial features increases intimidation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(4), 747–754.
- 127. Sell A., Cosmides L., & Tooby J. (2014). The human anger face evolved to enhance cues of strength. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(5), 425–429.
- 128. Zebrowitz L.A., Franklin R.G., & Boshyan J. (2015). Face shape and behavior: Implications of similarities in infants and adults. Personality and Individual Differences 86: 312–317. pmid:26217067
- 129. Hehman E., Leitner J. B., & Freeman J. B. (2014). The face–time continuum: Lifespan changes in facial width-to-height ratio impact aging-associated perceptions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(12), 1624–1636. pmid:25278108
- 130. Yasui T., Yonetsu M., Tanaka R., Tanaka Y., Fukushima S. I., Yamashita T., et al. (2012). In vivo observation of age-related structural changes of dermal collagen in human facial skin using collagen-sensitive second harmonic generation microscope equipped with 1250-nm mode-locked Cr: Forsterite laser. Journal of biomedical optics, 18(3), 031108.
- 131. Shaw R. B. Jr, Katzel E. B., Koltz P. F., Yaremchuk M. J., Girotto J. A., Kahn D. M., et al. (2011). Aging of the facial skeleton: aesthetic implications and rejuvenation strategies. Plastic and reconstructive surgery, 127(1), 374–383. pmid:20871486
- 132. Stirrat M., Stulp G., & Pollet T. V. (2012). Male facial width is associated with death by contact violence: narrow-faced males are more likely to die from contact violence. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(5), 551–556.
- 133. Wong A., Woods M. G., & Stella D. (2016). Three-dimensional computed tomographic assessment of mandibular muscles in growing subjects with different vertical facial patterns. Australian orthodontic journal, 32(1), 2. pmid:27468586
- 134. Fitch W. T., & Giedd J. (1999). Morphology and development of the human vocal tract: A study using magnetic resonance imaging. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106(3), 1511–1522. pmid:10489707
- 135. Harries M. L., Walker J. M., Williams D. M., Hawkins S., & Hughes I. A. (1997). Changes in the male voice at puberty. Archives of disease in childhood, 77(5), 445–447. pmid:9487971
- 136. Arnocky S., Hodges-Simeon C. R., Ouellette D., & Albert G. (2018b). Do men with more masculine voices have better immunocompetence? Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(6), 602–610.
- 137. Landry N., Desrochers J. E., Hodges-Simeon C., & Arnocky S. (2019). Testosterone, facial and vocal masculinization and low environmentalism in men. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 64, 107–112.
- 138. Feinberg D. R., Jones B. C., Little A. C., Burt D. M., & Perrett D. I. (2005). Manipulations of fundamental and formant frequencies influence the attractiveness of human male voices. Animal behaviour, 69(3), 561–568.
- 139. Puts D. A., Hodges C. R., Cárdenas R. A., & Gaulin S. J. (2007). Men’s voices as dominance signals: vocal fundamental and formant frequencies influence dominance attributions among men. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(5), 340–344.
- 140. Pisanski K., Fraccaro P. J., Tigue C. C., O’Connor J. J., Röder S., Andrews P. W., et al. (2014). Vocal indicators of body size in men and women: a meta-analysis. Animal Behaviour, 95, 89–99.
- 141.
Kolar J. C., & Salter E. M. (1997). Craniofacial anthropometry: Practical measurement of the head and face for clinical, surgical, and research use. Charles C. Thomas Publisher.
- 142. Costa M., Lio G., Gomez A., & Sirigu A. (2017). How components of facial width to height ratio differently contribute to the perception of social traits. PloS one, 12(2), e0172739. pmid:28235081
- 143. Hill A. K., Hunt J., Welling L. L., Cárdenas R. A., Rotella M. A., Wheatley J. R.,… & Puts D. A. (2013). Quantifying the strength and form of sexual selection on men’s traits. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(5), 334–341.
- 144. Quist M. C., Watkins C. D., Smith F. G., DeBruine L. M., & Jones B. C. (2011). Facial masculinity is a cue to women’s dominance. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(7), 1089–1093.
- 145. Jones B. C., Feinberg D. R., DeBruine L. M., Little A. C., & Vukovic J. (2010). A domain-specific opposite-sex bias in human preferences for manipulated voice pitch. Animal Behaviour, 79(1), 57–62.
- 146. Brooks R. C., Shelly J. P., Jordan L. A., & Dixson B. J. (2015). The multivariate evolution of female body shape in an artificial digital ecosystem. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36, 351–358.
- 147. Janson K. T., Bleck K., Fenkl J., Riegl L. T., Jägel F., & Köllner M. G. (2018). Inhibited power motivation is associated with the facial width-to-height ratio in females. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 4(1), 21–41.
- 148. Kajonius P., & Eldblom H. (2020). Facial Width-to-Height Ratio as a Cue of Threat: An Initial Event-Related Potential Study. International Journal of Psychological and Brain Sciences, 5(1), 1–4.
- 149. Krenn B., & Buehler C. (2019). Facial features and unethical behavior–Doped athletes show higher facial width-to-height ratios than non-doping sanctioned athletes. PloS one, 14(10), e0224472. pmid:31665155
- 150. Krenn B., & Meier J. (2018). Does Facial Width-to-Height Ratio Predict Aggressive Behavior in Association Football? Evolutionary Psychology, 16(4), 1474704918818590.
- 151. Muñoz-Reyes J. A., Polo P., Valenzuela N., Pavez P., Ramírez-Herrera O., Figueroa O., et al. (2020). the Male Warrior Hypothesis: testosterone-related cooperation and Aggression in the context of Intergroup Conflict. Scientific reports, 10(1), 1–12. pmid:31913322
- 152. Rostovtseva V. V., Mezentseva A. A., Windhager S., & Butovskaya M. L. (2020). Sexual dimorphism in facial shape of modern Buryats of Southern Siberia. American Journal of Human Biology, e23458. pmid:32596969
- 153. Ahmed S., Sihvonen J., & Vähämaa S. (2019). CEO facial masculinity and bank risk-taking. Personality and Individual Differences, 138, 133–139.
- 154. Arnocky S., Carré J. M., Bird B. M., Moreau B. J., Vaillancourt T., Ortiz T., et al. (2018). The facial width-to-height ratio predicts sex drive, sociosexuality, and intended infidelity. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 47(5), 1375–1385. pmid:28929303
- 155. Burton C. M., & Rule N. O. (2013). Judgments of height from faces are informed by dominance and facial maturity. Social Cognition, 31(6), 672–685.
- 156. Carré J. M., Murphy K. R., & Hariri A. R. (2013). What lies beneath the face of aggression? Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 8(2), 224–229. pmid:22198969
- 157. Cleary S., Jona J., Lee G., & Shemesh J. (2020). Underlying risk preferences and analyst risk-taking behavior. Journal of Business Finance & Accounting.
- 158. Fawcett T. W., Ewans J., Lawrence A., & Radford A. N. (2019). Attractiveness is positively related to World Cup performance in male, but not female, biathletes. Behavioral Ecology, 30(5), 1436–1442.
- 159. Fuji T., Goto A., & Takagishi H. (2016). Does facial width-to-height ratio predict Japanese professional football players’ athletic performance? Letters on Evolutionary Behavioral Science, 7(1), 37–40.
- 160. Hahn T., Winter N. R., Anderl C., Notebaert K., Wuttke A. M., Clément C. C., et al. (2017). Facial width-to-height ratio differs by social rank across organizations, countries, and value systems. PLoS ONE, 8, 12(11), e0187957. pmid:29121113
- 161. Haselhuhn M. P., Wong E. M., Ormiston M. E., Inesi M. E., & Galinsky A. D. (2014). Negotiating face-to-face: Men’s facial structure predicts negotiation performance. The Leadership Quarterly, 25(5), 835–845.
- 162. Hehman E., Leitner J. B., Deegan M. P., & Gaertner S. L. (2015). Picking teams: When dominant facial structure is preferred. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 59, 51–59.
- 163. Hehman E., Leitner J. B., & Freeman J. B. (2014). The face–time continuum: Lifespan changes in facial width-to-height ratio impact aging-associated perceptions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(12), 1624–1636. pmid:25278108
- 164. Huh H., Yi D., & Zhu H. (2014). Facial width-to-height ratio and celebrity endorsements. Personality and Individual Differences, 68, 43–47.
- 165. Kakkar H., Sivanathan N., & Gobel M. S. (2020). Fall from grace: The role of dominance and prestige in the punishment of high-status actors. Academy of Management Journal, 63(2), 530–553.
- 166. Kamiya S., Kim Y. H., & Park S. (2019). The face of risk: CEO facial masculinity and firm risk. European Financial Management, 25(2), 239–270.
- 167. Lieberz K. A., Windmann S., Geniole S. N., McCormick C. M., Mueller‐Engelmann M., et al. (2017). The facial width‐to‐height ratio determines interpersonal distance preferences in the observer. Aggressive behavior, 43(5), 460–470. pmid:28261811
- 168. Mileva V. R., Cowan M. L., Cobey K. D., Knowles K. K., & Little A. C. (2014). In the face of dominance: Self-perceived and other-perceived dominance are positively associated with facial-width-to-height ratio in men. Personality and Individual Differences, 69, 115–118.
- 169. Ormiston M. E., Wong E. M., & Haselhuhn M. P. (2017). Facial-width-to-height ratio predicts perceptions of integrity in males. Personality and Individual Differences, 105, 40–42.
- 170. Palmer‐Hague J. L., Twele A. C., & Fuller A. J. (2018). Body mass index, facial width‐to‐height ratio, and perceived formidability in female Ultimate Fighting Championship (UFC) fighters. Aggressive Behavior, 44(6), 553–560. pmid:29956344
- 171. Price M. E., Sheehy-Skeffington J., Sidnaius J., & Pound N. (2017). Is sociopolitical egalitarianism related to bodily and facial formidability in men? Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(5), 626–634.
- 172. Wang D., Nair K., Kouchaki M., Zajac E. J., & Zhao X. (2019). A case of evolutionary mismatch? Why facial width-to-height ratio may not predict behavioral tendencies. Psychological science, 30(7), 1074–1081. pmid:31180794
- 173. Wen G., & Zheng L. (2020). Facial width to height ratio predicts physical aggression in committed relationships in men and dominance in women in China. Personality and Individual Differences, 157, 109832.
- 174. Yang Y., Tang C., Qu X., Wang C., & Denson T. F. (2018). Group Facial Width-to-Height Ratio Predicts Intergroup Negotiation Outcomes. Frontiers in psychology, 9, 214. pmid:29515511
- 175. Zhang D., Lin H., & Perrett D. I. (2020a). Apparent emotional expression explains the effects of head posture on perceived trustworthiness and dominance, but a measure of facial width does not. Perception, 49(4), 422–438.
- 176. Cunningham M., Barbee A., & Pike C. (1990). What Do Women Want? Facialmetric Assessment of Multiple Motives in the Perception of Male Facial Physical Attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(1), 61–72. pmid:2213490
- 177. Grammer K., & Thornhill R. (1994). Human (Homo sapiens) facial attractiveness and sexual selection: the role of symmetry and averageness. Journal of comparative psychology, 108(3), 233. pmid:7924253
- 178. Koehler N., Simmons L. W., Rhodes G., & Peters M. (2004). The relationship between sexual dimorphism in human faces and fluctuating asymmetry. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 271, S233–S236. pmid:15252993
- 179. Little A. C., Burriss R. P., Petrie M., Jones B. C., & Roberts S. C. (2013). Oral contraceptive use in women changes preferences for male facial masculinity and is associated with partner facial masculinity. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(9), 1777–1785. pmid:23528282
- 180. Mogilski J. K., & Welling L. L. (2018). The relative contribution of jawbone and cheekbone prominence, eyebrow thickness, eye size, and face length to evaluations of facial masculinity and attractiveness: A conjoint data-driven approach. Frontiers in psychology, 9.
- 181. Scheib J. E., Gangestad S. W., & Thornhill R. (1999). Facial attractiveness, symmetry and cues of good genes. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 266(1431), 1913–1917. pmid:10535106
- 182. Wade T. J. (2016). Does skin color, facial shape, and facial Width to Height Ratio (fWHR) play a role in Black male facial evaluation. EvoS Journal: The Journal of the Evolutionary Studies Consortium, 22.
- 183. Alrajih S., & Ward J. (2014). Increased facial width‐to‐height ratio and perceived dominance in the faces of the UK’s leading business leaders. British Journal of Psychology, 105(2), 153–161. pmid:24754804
- 184. Anderl C., Hahn T., Schmidt A. K., Moldenhauer H., Notebaert K., Clément C. C., et al. (2016). Facial width-to-height ratio predicts psychopathic traits in males. Personality and Individual Differences, 88, 99–101.
- 185. Chan K. H., Wang R. R., & Wang R. (2020). The Macbeth Factor: The Dark Side of Achievement-driving Analysts. Abacus.
- 186. Efferson C., & Vogt S. (2013). Viewing men’s faces does not lead to accurate predictions of trustworthiness. Scientific Reports, 3(4), 1047. pmid:23308340
- 187. He X., Yin H., Zeng Y., Zhang H., & Zhao H. (2019). Facial structure and achievement drive: Evidence from financial analysts. Journal of Accounting Research, 57(4), 1013–1057.
- 188. Lebuda I., & Karwowski M. (2016). Written on the Writer’s Face: Facial Width-to-Height Ratio among Nominees and Laureates of the Nobel Prize in Literature. Creativity Research Journal, 28(2), 207–211.
- 189. Żelaźniewicz A., Nowak J., & Pawłowski B. (2020). Birth size and morphological femininity in adult women. BMC Evolutionary Biology, 20(1), 1–8. pmid:31906845
- 190. Zhang W., Hahn A. C., Cai Z., Lee A. J., Holzleitner I. J., DeBruine L. M., et al. (2018). No evidence that facial width-to-height ratio (fWHR) is associated with women’s sexual desire. PloS one, 13(7), e0200308. pmid:30020967
- 191. Lee A. J., Wright M. J., Martin N. G., Keller M. C., & Zietsch B. P. (2017). Facial trustworthiness is associated with heritable aspects of face shape. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 3(4), 351–364.
- 192. Polo P., Muñoz‐Reyes J. A., Pita M., Shackelford T. K., & Fink B. (2019). Testosterone‐dependent facial and body traits predict men’s sociosexual attitudes and behaviors. American Journal of Human Biology, 31(3), e23235. pmid:30884051