Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Intolerance of loud sounds in childhood: Is there an intergenerational association with grandmaternal smoking in pregnancy?

  • Amanda Hall,

    Roles Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Life and Health Sciences, Aston University, Birmingham, England, United Kingdom

  • Kate Northstone,

    Roles Data curation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Bristol Medical School (Public Health Sciences), University of Bristol, Bristol, England, United Kingdom

  • Yasmin Iles-Caven,

    Roles Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Centre for Academic Child Health, Bristol Medical School (Public Health Sciences), University of Bristol, Bristol, England, United Kingdom

  • Genette Ellis,

    Roles Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Centre for Academic Child Health, Bristol Medical School (Public Health Sciences), University of Bristol, Bristol, England, United Kingdom

  • Steve Gregory,

    Roles Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Centre for Academic Child Health, Bristol Medical School (Public Health Sciences), University of Bristol, Bristol, England, United Kingdom

  • Jean Golding ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    jean.golding@bristol.ac.uk

    Affiliation Centre for Academic Child Health, Bristol Medical School (Public Health Sciences), University of Bristol, Bristol, England, United Kingdom

  • Marcus Pembrey

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Centre for Academic Child Health, Bristol Medical School (Public Health Sciences), University of Bristol, Bristol, England, United Kingdom

Abstract

Recent research using the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) demonstrated an association between maternal grandmother smoking in pregnancy and the autistic traits of impaired social communication and repetitive behaviour in granddaughters but not grandsons, but of paternal grandmother smoking and early development of myopia in the grandchild. Here we investigate whether grandmaternal smoking in pregnancy is associated with intolerance to loud sounds. ALSPAC collected information during the index pregnancy from the study parents on the smoking habits, social and other features of their own parents. Maternal report when the child was aged 6 and 13 included hating loud sounds; at age 11 the child was tested for volume preference for listening to music through headphones. Statistical analysis compared results for grandchildren in relation to whether a parent had been exposed in utero to maternal smoking, adjusted for their grandparents’ social and demographic attributes. We hypothesised that there would be sex differences in the effects of grandmaternal prenatal smoking, based on previous intergenerational studies. For 6-year-old children maternal report of intolerance to loud noise was more likely in grandsons if the maternal grandmother had smoked [adjusted odds ratio (AOR) 1.27; 95% confidence interval (CI) 1.03,1.56; P = 0.025], but less likely in girls [AOR 0.82; 95%CI 0.63,1.07] Pinteraction <0.05. If the paternal grandmother had smoked the grandchildren were less likely to be intolerant, especially girls. The objective measure of choice of volume for music through headphones showed that grandsons of both maternal and paternal smoking grandmothers were less likely to choose high volumes compared with granddaughters (P<0.05). In line with our prior hypothesis of sex differences, we showed that grandsons were more intolerant of loud sounds than granddaughters particularly at age 6, and this was confirmed by objective measures at age 11.

Background

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) birth cohort was established in 1991 to study developmental phenotypic variation across the local population in relation to inherited DNA variants and a wide range of dietary, social and environmental exposures. [1] Maternal exposures during and before the study pregnancy was one interest during the first decade, being relevant to the Developmental Origins of Health and Disease (DOHaD) hypothesis [2,3] and the emergence of environmental epigenetics [4,5]. By the second decade, ALSPAC began exploring associations with grandmaternal exposures in the light of experimental evidence of mammalian epigenetic inheritance across several generations (see below). Whilst population-wide traits have been the primary ALSPAC outcome measures, doctor-diagnoses are recorded and, in common multifactorial conditions, the two may be considered together. Two recent examples of this are postnatal refractive eye development focusing on refractive errors underlying myopia (near sightedness) [6] and four childhood behavioural traits predictive of autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and diagnosed ASD [7].

Since the launch of human genome wide association studies (GWAS) in 2007 [8] the mainstream view of the inheritance of complex traits and multifactorial diseases is that the heritability will eventually be explained by the sum total of common and increasingly rare DNA sequence variations inherited by the individual, plus ‘de novo’ mutations arising early in development. As whole genome sequencing takes off, there is some support for this view with respect to the genetics of height [9], no support from studies of Type 2 diabetes [10,11] and a speculative reappraisal of what complex traits actually are [12]. In parallel, the last decade has seen mammalian evidence of non-DNA-sequence-based inheritance induced by parental/ancestral experiences [13]. These effects are called intergenerational if the exposure (or importantly the organism’s response to it, e.g. response to DNA damage) could have reached the germ cells leading to the next generation(s), or transgenerational if this is not the case. The latter implies some molecular ‘memory’ of the ancestral experience, or its response to it, is being passed down via the gametes. We have written inter/trans-generational when both apply.

The molecular basis of intergenerational or transgenerational epigenetic inheritance is poorly understood, particularly in humans, but it does include altered DNA methylation, histone modification and exposure induced non-coding RNAs being transferred to sperm [14,15]. Thus, what has, in the past, been interpreted as ‘likely to be genetic’, is now better considered more broadly ‘as likely to be factors inherited at conception’[16]. In line with this view, ALSPAC has included assessment of associations between parental/ancestral early-life experiences and developmental variation in the study participants. Unlike initial human epidemiological studies by others of associations with parental/ancestral exposure to historic famines [17,18] we selected cigarette smoking in the early 2000’s as a suitable exposure for proof of principle studies in a contemporary population.

As described in detail elsewhere [19] the intention at the start was to see if we could replicate some of the distinctive features of human intergenerational and transgenerational associations found by others, principally the exposure sensitive period(s) during development. We found support for the mid-childhood sensitive period, initially shown by Bygren and colleagues [18], and noted a sex-specific bias in the intergenerational association with paternal onset of regular smoking before 11years. This led to a collaboration with Professor Bygren and colleagues to test for sex-specific effects in the Swedish data; and striking sex-specific (but not sex-limited) associations were found [20]. In view of this, we started a programme on intergenerational association studies within ALSPAC with smoking as the exposure of interest. We began by analysing the child’s growth and anthropomorphic measures in relation to study fathers who started regular smoking before puberty (<11ys) [21] and with either grandmother smoking in pregnancy [22]. We have since extended these intergenerational studies to behavioural and sensory perception measures collected prospectively in the study participants.

The hypersensitivity measures were intolerance of loud sounds and the ability to detect bitter taste using 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP) [23]. Here we consider intolerance of loud sounds in relation to grandmaternal smoking. Aetiological studies of intolerance to loud sounds per se have rarely been undertaken but may be incorporated into studies of hyperacusis; namely intolerance to everyday or low intensity sounds. Hyperacusis is a feature of certain monogenic disorders, such as Williams syndrome [24] and Smith-Megenis syndrome [25] and occurs in about 20% of children with the multifactorial disorder ASD [26,27]. In adults with hyperacusis who are otherwise neurotypical, the medial olivocochlear (MOC) efferent reflex is stronger than usual, as it is in children with ASD and coexisting hyperacusis [28]. Among the candidate genes for ASD, the contactin genes CNTN5 and CNTN6 code for neural cell adhesion proteins that promote neurite outgrowth and synaptogenesis.[29]. CNTN6 mutations, in particular, are risk factors for abnormal auditory sensory perception amongst those with ASD [30]. Our earlier finding of an association between maternal grandmother smoking in pregnancy and impaired social communication and repetitive behaviour in granddaughters but not grandsons [7] provides an example of intergenerational responses to maternal grandmaternal smoking in pregnancy. However, the present study of intolerance to loud sounds was not intended or designed to focus on ASD.

There is a substantial experimental animal literature on the ways in which specific environmental exposures can influence the phenotype of subsequent (unexposed) generations through non-DNA sequence-based inheritance [13,31,32]. In relevant animal experiments exposures are often delivered to a pregnant dam thereby exposing her offspring throughout fetal life. The dam is traditionally labelled F0, so we have adopted the same notation for the study grandmothers, F1 for their offspring–the study parents, and F2 for the study participants. The results of animal experiments support the human epidemiological observations and provides numerous examples of sex-specificity or sex-bias in both the route of transmission down the generations and the phenotypic outcomes in descendants. As mentioned above, sex-specificity was a feature of initial human studies and this is true of early rodent experiments [33,34,35].

The transmissions (progressing from intergenerational to transgenerational) can be complicated as illustrated by the study of maternal high-fat diet exposure in mice [36]. This resulted in an increase in body size and reduced insulin sensitivity in both sexes that persisted across two generations (F1 & F2) via both maternal and paternal lineages. However, at the next generation (F3) only the females had increased body size and this effect was only passed on via the paternal lineage. Another example is a study that induced chronic social instability in adolescent mice (F0) and then followed their unexposed descendants (F1, F2 & F3) [37]. Only female descendants exhibited anxiety-like behaviour and social deficits, but after F1 this phenotype is only transmitted through males who themselves do not manifest anxiety-like behaviour and social deficits. This latter study was the starting point for a commentary that explored why sex matters in such intergenerational and transgenerational responses.[38] It could not provide any conclusive explanations for the sex-specificity but recognised that the phenomenon was common. It is important to emphasise that intergenerational phenotypic responses can be widespread, for example combine metabolic and behavioural features [39] and include beneficial outcomes [40].

There is not necessarily transmission of the initial phenotype, although inheritance of an acquired characteristics can occur. Sometimes the F1 or F2 phenotypic can change to be in the opposite direction, as if the response is a corrective adaptation [41]. Increasingly animal experiments permit organ/cell specific gene expression and epigenomic studies. These can include germline, sperm, ovum or early embryo analyses to elucidate the molecular mechanisms that mediate intergenerational transmissions [31,32,42]. Such opportunities are limited in humans, although there have been ingenious studies to elucidate the nature of the epigenome in the conceptus and its trajectory into the human pre-implantation embryo [43].

Overall human results from epidemiological, observational studies over the last two decades have been in line with rodent experiments (reviewed in [44]). The inter/trans-generational responses can be through either the paternal or maternal line; and like rodent experiments, human observations often demonstrated a sex-specificity (but not necessarily sex limitation) in either the parental route of transmission and/or the offspring phenotypic outcomes. This is true of most of the human studies referenced below since 2005 and why we have hypothesised that there will be sex-specific differences in any transgenerational associations detected in this study of intolerance to loud sounds. Human studies have defined ‘exposure-sensitive periods’ during early development; namely fetal life and mid-childhood just before puberty. These exposure times are broadly supported by responses to swings in F0 food supply [17,19,33,45,46] or F0 smoking [21,47,48, 49]. However, some F0 pre-conceptional exposures in (early) adulthood can also be associated with phenotypic changes in the next generation [21].

Exposure to famine is likely to be confounded by psychological stress, and F0 exposure to stress per se has also been reported to be associated with altered psychological state or early development in the next generation [50,51]. As described above, smoking was selected in the early 2000’s as an exposure (with available data) in a contemporary population with which to valid the exposure sensitive periods of fetal life and mid-childhood as indicated by studies of historic famines. Smoking can clearly initiate intergenerational responses, which raises the question as to how it can induce the relevant molecular change(s) in the gametes or the resulting conceptus. It is important to note that tobacco smoking causes DNA damage [52]. This damage or the body’s response to it [53] may well be the key ‘signal’ that is transmitted to the next generation(s); and with such a generic genomic stress, the impact on development may be widespread.

Material and methods

The study subjects

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) is an ongoing study which started during 1990–1992 by attempting to enrol all pregnant women resident in a defined area of Avon (in south-west England) with expected date of delivery between 1st April 1991 and 31st December 1992. Avon comprises the city of Bristol, surrounding urban and semi-urban areas, as well as rural communities. It has a total population of about 1 million people. A variety of methods were used to encourage enrolment in the study, using the health services, local publicity, contacts through chemists (drug stores), libraries and mother and toddler groups [54]. Approximately 80% of the eligible population took part in the study. The initial number of pregnancies enrolled is 14,541 (for these at least one questionnaire had been returned or a “Children in Focus” clinic had been attended by the 19th July 1999). Of these initial pregnancies, there was a total of 14,676 foetuses, resulting in 14,062 live births and 13,988 children who were alive at 1 year of age. Comparison of the population enrolled with the census records of families with babies resident in the area in 1991 showed that the ALSPAC population was fairly representative although there was a slight bias in that those from the less well educated group were under-represented [54,55,56].

Data were collected using a variety of methods, the most relevant to this research were: (a) self-completion questionnaires to the parents, answered in their own homes and returned to the study centre by post; (b) annual hands-on examinations of the children from the age of 7; (c) blood collected for genetic and epigenetic studies at different time points. The study website contains details of all the data that is available through a fully searchable data dictionary and variable search tool: http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/our-data/. Data are available to bona fide applicants provided that by doing so no ethical or confidentiality guidelines are broken.

Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee [ALEC; IRB00003312] (registered on the Office of Human Research Protections database as U Bristol IRB #1). ALEC agreed that consent was implied if questionnaires were returned. Informed written consent was obtained for all biological samples prior to analysis, and for certain invasive procedures during the hands-on assessments (which were optional to attend) from the participant and/or legal guardian [57]. All study methods were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. Together with the local Health Services ethics committees ALEC has approved the linkage of the DNA and methylation data to the detailed assessments and other information on the parents and children. Analyses of biological samples including genetic and DNA methylation are only carried out for individuals for whom informed generic consent has been received. Further detailed information on the ways in which confidentiality of the cohort is maintained may be found on the study website: http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/research-ethics/

The auditory outcome variables

Intolerance to loud sound was determined through parental questionnaire. When the child was 81 months (6 years) old, the mother was sent a questionnaire which included the question: ‘Does he/she prefer music or talking to be loud or soft? with possible answers (i) She hates loud sounds; (ii) He/She doesn’t mind if it’s loud or not; (iii) He/She loves loud sounds; (iv) Can’t say. In all 8391 answers were received (an additional 124 were not completed). The frequency of answers was: (i) 10.9%; (ii) 73.7%; (iii) 11.4% and (iv) 3.9%. For this study we have compared those who hated loud sounds (10.9%) with all other children (89.1%).

At the age of 157 months (13 years), an identical question was asked of the mother in regard to her child. There were 7094 valid responses and an additional 71 where the question had been left blank. The frequency of answers was (i) 5.4%; (ii) 72.7%; (iii) 17.2% and (iv) 4.7%. The analyses in this paper compare those who hated loud sounds (5.4%) with the remainder (94.6%).

As a validation analysis we used an objective measure of the child’s comfortable loudness listening level measured when the children were age 11. Those who attended the hearing session of the Focus 11 clinic, were played a short piece of music through a portable CD player and supra-aural, Sennheiser PX40 headphones. They were asked to set the CD player at the volume level at which they would typically listen to music. The music was composed specifically for the purpose and thus had not previously been heard by the children. The sound pressure level of the CD player and headphones was calibrated using a KEMAR manikin (head and torso simulator) to give the equivalent sound pressure level at the eardrum for each volume setting. Each increment on the volume setting was equivalent to a change of 0.9–1.2 dB.

The volume level set by the child was noted. We identified the binary variable of the highest 10% of scores as a measure of tolerance to loud sounds, with the expectation that those who are intolerant to loud sounds would choose a lower volume level on the headphone test. The top 10% were listening on average at volume levels between 79 and 85 dB (A), where 85 dB (A) is equivalent to the sound of a noisy restaurant or food blender. The UK Control of Noise at Work Regulations (2005) state that exposure to noise of 85 dB (A) or above in the workplace (daily or weekly average exposure) is the level of sound at which employers must provide ear protection for their workers [58]. Consequently, we expected those who declared that they hated loud sounds to be under-represented in this group. A direct comparison showed that the mean volume level chosen by 388 11-year-olds who were said to hate loud noises at age 6 was significantly lower than that of the 3113 who did not hate such noises (mean difference -0.538; SE 0.314; P <0.05). A comparison of the 182 who were said at age 13 to hate loud noises, with the remaining 3295 showed a larger mean difference (-2.912; SE 0.445; P<0.0001), equivalent to around 3 dB quieter, thus indicating that the listening level measure was more closely aligned with the 13-year-old outcome.

Hearing function was assessed at age 11. Air conduction hearing thresholds were measured at 0.5, 1, 2 and 4 kHz, in both the left and right ears using a GSI 61 audiometer in a sound treated booth. Average hearing thresholds across 0.5–4 kHz were calculated for both ears. Ipsilateral acoustic reflexes were measured using a GSI 38 tympanometer: on completion of tympanometry the presence of ipsilaterally evoked acoustic reflexes were measured at 85, 95, and 105 dB. The left ear was tested first and the lowest level sound at which the reflex was evoked was recorded. If no reflex was present it was recorded as absent.

The exposures

Within one of the questionnaires that was sent to each study parent (F1) during pregnancy, was a question as to whether their own mothers (F0) had smoked, and if so whether they had smoked during the pregnancy that resulted in their own birth. Where the parent knew that their mother had smoked but had not known whether she had smoked during the pregnancy, we assumed that she had done so. We have shown elsewhere, by looking at the study mother’s own birthweight that this was a valid assumption since the mean birthweight of the mothers who had been exposed as fetuses was 148g less than that of the women born to women we have assumed not to have smoked in pregnancy [14]. Using this definition of grandmother (F0) smoking in pregnancy results in a prevalence of fetal exposure for our study mothers and fathers (F1) of 37.9% and 42.1% respectively.

Potential confounders

We examined the backgrounds of each of the grandparents (F0) in regard to each of the outcomes listed above. We compared their years of birth, their ages at the birth of their offspring (F1), their ethnic background (white; non-white), education level (the equivalent of ≥O-level; < O-level); their ages at the birth of the parent; the grandmother’s parity at the birth of the study parent (no previous children; previous children); the social class based on their occupations, categorised using the UK standard classification system [59]; whether they were smokers. We selected the variables identified as significantly associated with each specific outcome indicating reaction to loud sounds to be potential confounders.

Statistical analyses

Initial analyses compared the unadjusted risk of each grandchild (F2) outcome with each of the parental fetal exposures. This was repeated, selecting whether the mothers themselves were smokers or non-smokers, and whether the grandchild was a boy or girl. Data were displayed as odds ratios with 95% confidence intervals.

For statistical adjustments, comparison was made of the risk of each potential confounder with each outcome, and those variables that had a P-value of < 0.10 were selected. These variables were then included in the logistic regression analyses according to whether the exposures were associated with the mother’s or father’s line. The variable concerning the relevant prenatal smoking of the grandmother was then incorporated into each analysis. The process was repeated according to whether the mother (F1) herself smoked or not, and for the sex of the grandchildren (F2). (We used the cut-point of P<0.10 in order not to omit any interesting or important associations throughout the paper).

Results

Responses

The proportion of study pregnancies that resulted in a child for whom the questions on loud sound intolerance were answered at ages 6 and 13 are shown in S1 Table, together with the proportion who attended the clinic at age 11 and performed the listening test. This shows a small difference in the likelihood of there being information available on the child (F2) according to whether the grandmother (F0) had smoked in pregnancy or not (e.g. for 6-year-old outcomes information was available for 57.4% of children whose maternal grandmothers had smoked prenatally v. 60.8% of children whose grandmother had not smoked in pregnancy). The same pattern was apparent for paternal grandmothers smoking in pregnancy and for each outcome–the information being slightly more likely to be available if the grandmother had not smoked.

Maternal grandmother smoking and child intolerance to loud sound

The variation in unadjusted odds ratios for intolerance to loud sounds for all children showed little sign of any association with maternal grandmother smoking (Table 1). However, there was an increase in odds among the boys at age 6, especially when the mother did not smoke (P<0.10). Although the odds ratios for the girls were no different than would be expected by chance, the difference in odds between boys and girls showed a significant interaction (P < 0.05). There were no associations with intolerance to loud sounds at age 13.

thumbnail
Table 1. Unadjusted risk of the child hating loud sounds at ages 6 and 13 if the maternal grandmother had smoked in pregnancy.

[P values <0.10 are in bold].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229323.t001

Examination of the variables that were associated with intolerance to loud noises at 6 (Table 2 and S2 Table) identified the year of birth of each maternal grandparent (F0), the age of her father (F0) at the time of birth of the study mother (F1), and the ethnic background of each of her parents (F0) as potential confounders.

thumbnail
Table 2. Proportion (n) of children who hated loud noises at age 6 according to features of their grandparents.

[P values <0.10 are in bold].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229323.t002

Once these were taken into account (Table 3), the associations for boys at age 6 became significant at P<0.05, and there were still significant interactions between the sexes with the girls being less likely to be intolerant to loud noises and the boys more so. This was true for all children as well as for those whose mothers did not smoke. In contrast there were no such associations for the children who were intolerant to loud noises at age 13.

thumbnail
Table 3. Adjusted risk of the child hating loud sounds at ages 6 and 13 if the maternal grandmother had smoked in pregnancy.

[P values <0.10 are in bold]. Adjusted for year of birth of each grandparent, age of grandfather at birth of the mother, ethnic origins of both grandparents.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229323.t003

Paternal grandmother smoking and child intolerance to loud sounds

The patterns were quite different if the paternal grandmother had smoked in pregnancy (Table 4). The unadjusted data for intolerance to loud noises at age 6 showed a reduced risk for all children and for girls, as well as for the children whose mothers did not smoke. Again, there were significant differences between the sexes, with the girls being less likely than the boys to be intolerant of loud noises (P<0.05). At age 13, all children were significantly more likely to have a reduced risk, with boys and girls having similar odds ratios. This was true of those whose mothers did not smoke, but when the mother did smoke, there was a significant difference between the sexes–the girls being more likely than the boys to be intolerant to loud noises.

thumbnail
Table 4. Unadjusted risk of the child hating loud sounds at ages 6 and 13 if the paternal grandmother had smoked in pregnancy.

[P values <0.10 are in bold].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229323.t004

Examination of the variables related to paternal grandparents that were associated with intolerance to loud noises at 6 and 13 (Table 2 and S2 Table) identified the year of birth of each of the grandparents (F0), the age of the grandparents (F0) at the time of birth of the study father (F1), and the ethnic background of each of his parents (F0) as potential confounders. The adjusted odds for children intolerant to loud noises were still reduced for all children but there were no longer interactions between the sexes with the exception of the group where the study mother (F1) had smoked: as in the unadjusted data at age 13, the girls were more likely than the boys to be intolerant to loud sounds (Table 5).

thumbnail
Table 5. Adjusted risk of the child hating loud sounds at ages 6 and 13 if the paternal grandmother had smoked in pregnancy.

[P values <0.10 are in bold]. Adjusted for year of birth of each paternal grandparent, age of grandfather at birth of the father, ethnic origins of both grandparents.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229323.t005

Grandchild’s setting of music listening level.

The test of music listening level was repeated on two occasions (named run 1 and run 2). In order to determine whether choice of run made any difference to the results we compared the two sets of unadjusted mean results. They showed the same associations–we therefore decided to use Run 1 for further analyses since there were marginally more results for this than for Run 2 (S3A and S3B Table). The comparisons showed that the grandchildren whose grandmothers had smoked when expecting either parent tended to set their volume level higher than those whose grandmothers had not smoked in pregnancy, and that the granddaughters set their level higher than the grandsons.

In order to compare our results with those who were intolerant loud noise, we have analysed the volume listening results assuming that those who were intolerant of loud sounds would be less likely to choose the highest 10% volume levels. The unadjusted results for maternal grandmothers smoking in pregnancy (Table 6) indicate that the grandchildren, especially the granddaughters, were more likely to choose a louder volume. As before, we assessed the backgrounds of the grandparents to identify what variables to allow for (S4 Table).

thumbnail
Table 6. Odds ratio showing risk of stereo run 1 volume level being in the top 10% where maternal grandmother smoked.

Adjustment A concerns allowing for maternal grandmother’s year of birth, her ethnicity, her education level; Adjustment B additionally takes the maternal grandfather’s social class into account.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229323.t006

The factors that were relevant for the maternal line were the maternal grandmother’s year of birth, her ethnicity, her education level and the maternal grandfather’s social class. The relationships without and then including social class are shown in Table 6. Whether adjusted or not, there were significant interactions between the sexes–granddaughters whose mother had been exposed as a fetus to their grandmother smoking were significantly more likely to choose loud sounds than the grandsons, who tended to be less likely to make this choice. This was true of the population as a whole as well as of those grandchildren whose mothers did not smoke. If there was any association among grandchildren whose mothers smoked in pregnancy, the numbers of children involved were too small to demonstrate this.

For the paternal line, the factors adjusted for were the paternal grandfather’s year of birth, his highest educational qualification, and his social class. Although the results showed few unadjusted associations, again there were significant interactions, with granddaughters more likely to choose loud sounds and boys less so (Table 7).

thumbnail
Table 7. Odds ratio showing risk of stereo run 1 volume level being in the top 10% where paternal grandmother smoked.

Adjustment A concerns allowing for paternal grandmother’s year of birth, her ethnicity, and her education level; Adjustment B additionally takes the paternal grandfather’s social class into account.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229323.t007

Grandchild’s hearing function

There were no significant associations between either maternal or paternal grandmother’s smoking and auditory reflex threshold, hearing threshold, tinnitus or whether the child had ever been referred for hearing loss (S4S6 Tables).

Discussion

Our aim was to assess whether the grandchild’s intolerance to loud sounds had an intergenerational association with the grandmother’s smoking during the pregnancy that had resulted in the birth of the grandchild’s parent. We compared the grandchildren whose grandmothers smoked during pregnancy with the grandchildren whose grandmothers did not, analysing separately the maternal and paternal grandmothers. As in our previous studies [6, 21, 22], we considered the population as a whole as well as subdividing by sex of the grandchild and by whether the grandchild’s mother had smoked or not during the pregnancy.

We showed that about 11% of the 6-year-old grandchildren were said to be intolerant to loud sound. If the maternal grandmother had smoked in pregnancy the boys were more likely than expected, and the girls were less likely than expected to be so (P for interaction <0.05). This pattern was found for all grandchildren considered together, as well as for those whose mothers did not themselves smoke in pregnancy. There were no such relationships for 13-year-old grandchildren (Tables 1 and 3).

For paternal grandmothers’ smoking, the grandchildren tended to be less likely than expected to be intolerant of loud noises at both age 6 and 13, but the only interaction between the sexes was in the relatively small group comprising both mother and paternal grandmother smoking prenatally (the 13-year-old grandsons were less likely and the granddaughters more likely to hate loud sounds (Tables 4 and 5)). However, with much larger numbers, the granddaughters of grandmothers who had and mothers who had not smoked prenatally were significantly less likely to have such a reaction to loud noises.

Since the report of intolerance to loud sounds was based on subjective reports from parents, we used the preferred volume listening level set by the child themselves as a more objective measure. We assumed that children who were intolerant to loud sounds would be less likely to choose the loudest part of the register. As predicted, we found that, if the grandmother had smoked in pregnancy, not only were the grandsons more likely than the granddaughters to be said to be intolerant to loud sounds; confirmatory evidence was shown on testing–the grandsons were less likely than the granddaughters to choose to listen at higher volume levels (Tables 6 and 7).

There was no association of peripheral auditory function with grandmother’s smoking, indicating that the observed effect is not likely to be explained by differences in hearing thresholds. The question about sound intolerance asked in this study specifically relates to loud sounds, rather than intolerance to everyday or low intensity sounds (known as hyperacusis). Intolerance to loud sounds may relate to the phenomenon of loudness recruitment, a characteristic of cochlear hearing loss [60]. However, the lack of an association with hearing threshold suggests this is not a peripheral, cochlear phenomenon. The findings are consistent with models of hyperacusis which propose a central rather than peripheral cause, resulting from increased central auditory gain [61]. However as noted above, the measure used in this paper is not synonymous with hyperacusis [62].

At present we are largely ignorant of the causal pathways underpinning this and similar intergenerational responses to grandmaternal smoking in pregnancy. As indicated in the Introduction, the exposure of either (F1) parent to tobacco as a fetus may have resulted in a direct xenobiotic exposure to both their developing somatic tissues and their emerging germline, ultimately destined for any (F2) grandchildren. Alternatively, it may be the generalised DNA damage caused by (FO) grandmaternal smoking and/or the consequent DNA damage response of the fetus that modifies the emerging germline and alters F2 embryonic brain development. Clearly very complex, it is premature to speculate further.

Explaining the sex differences observed here, and in those reported in many earlier studies of inter/trans-generational responses, might seem more tractable. However, a recent lengthy review of the role of sex in the genomics of human complex traits emphasises that this has been a neglected topic [63]. These authors note that nearly all human complex traits and disease phenotypes exhibit some degree of sex differences, but attempts to attribute this solely to classic genetic x environmental effects has proved unfruitful. Despite devoting some space to epigenome analysis and genomic imprinting, the authors only reviewed single generation studies and assumed throughout that biological inheritance is purely down to transmission of DNA sequence differences. A meta-analysis of 2,335,920 twin pairs and 2,608 phenotypes reports that only 3% demonstrated significant sex differences in heritability [64]. Furthermore, classic twin studies are likely to be confounded by epigenetic super-similarity in monozygotic twins [43]. One conclusion from the failure of these huge studies to explain little of the sex biases in complex diseases and traits is that non-DNA sequence-based inheritance may be an important source of sex differences in developmental traits.

Strengths and limitations

The strengths of this study concern: (a) the fact that it is population based with a large number of subjects; (b) the fact that data on the grandparents were collected during pregnancy, long before the child’s sensitivity to loud sound could be identified; (c) the analysis was structured around the hypothesis that, like previous intergenerational associations with grandparental prenatal smoking, any associations observed in the grandchildren would show sex differences; (d) the outcome measures used were able to be verified with an objective measure (the volume which the grandchild set the CD player).

Limitations concern the fact that information on the child’s intolerance to loud sound are answered by the mother, and consequently are likely to depend on her own observational skills, and the general noise level in the home. However, this criticism is not valid for the direct measures of sound volume preferences using the headphones. A further limitation concerns the fact that the smoking history of the grandmother was collected retrospectively; nevertheless comparison of the birthweights of the daughters of the grandmothers reported as smoking prenatally with those who were not indicates that these data have validity. The major limitation of the study concerns the fact that, at this point in time, there is no evidence (confirmatory or otherwise) from other studies.

Supporting information

S1 Table. The proportion of study pregnancies that resulted in a child for whom the questions on sound intolerance were answered at ages 6 and 13 together with the proportion for whom the stereo test results were available according to whether the parent was born to a grandmother who smoked during their pregnancy.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229323.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Proportion (n) of children who hated loud noises at age 13 according to features of their grandparents.

[P values <0.10 are in bold].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229323.s002

(DOCX)

S3 Table.

a. Unadjusted mean stereo by maternal grandmother smoked. b. Unadjusted mean stereo by paternal grandmother smoked. [P values <0.10 are in bold].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229323.s003

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Highest 10% of stereo level chosen by the children at age 11 according to features of their grandparents.

[P values <0.10 are in bold].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229323.s004

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Risk of the child having a flat acoustic reflex at age 11 if the maternal grandmother had smoked in pregnancy.

[P values <0.10 are in bold].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229323.s005

(DOCX)

S6 Table. The prevalences of hearing features of the children at age 11 according to whether a grandmother had smoked prenatally when expecting the study parent.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229323.s006

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses.

References

  1. 1. Golding J, Pembrey M, Jones R. ALSPAC—the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. I. Study methodology. Paediatr Perinatal Epidemiol. 2001;15(1): 74–87.
  2. 2. Barker DJ. The fetal and infant origins of adult disease. BMJ.1990; 301, 1111. pmid:2252919
  3. 3. Gluckman PD, Hanson MA, Cooper C, Thornburg KL. Effect of in utero and early-life conditions on adult health and disease. N Engl J Med. 2008; 359, 61–73. pmid:18596274
  4. 4. Jirtle RL, Sander M, Barrett JC. Genomic imprinting and environmental disease susceptibility. Environ Health Perspect. 2000 Mar;108(3):271–8. pmid:10706535
  5. 5. Petronis A. The origin of schizophrenia: genetic thesis, epigenetic antithesis, and resolving synthesis. Biol Psychiatry. 2004 May 15;55(10):965–70. pmid:15121478
  6. 6. Williams C., Suderman M., Guggenheim J. A., Ellis G., Gregory S., Iles-Caven Y., et al. (2019). Grandmothers’ smoking in pregnancy is associated with a reduced prevalence of early-onset myopia. Scientific reports, 9(1), 1–12.
  7. 7. Golding J, Ellis G, Gregory S, Birmingham K, Iles-Caven Y, Rai D, et al. Grand-maternal smoking in pregnancy and grandchild’s autistic traits and diagnosed autism. Sci Rep. 2017;7: 46179 pmid:28448061
  8. 8. Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium. Genome-wide association study of 14,000 cases of seven common diseases and 3,000 shared controls. Nature. 2007 Jun 7;447(7145):661–78. pmid:17554300
  9. 9. Marouli E, Graff M, Medina-Gomez C, Lo KS, Wood AR, Kjaer TR, et al. Rare and low-frequency coding variants alter human adult height. Nature. 2017; 542(7640):186–190. [PubMed: pmid:28146470]
  10. 10. Rich SS. Diabetes: Still a geneticist's nightmare. Nature 2016; 536(7614):37–8. pmid:27398618
  11. 11. Fuchsberger et al, The genetic architecture of type 2 diabetes. Nature. 2016 Aug 4;536(7614):41–47. pmid:27398621
  12. 12. Boyle EA, Li YI, & Pritchard JK. (2017). An Expanded View of Complex Traits: From Polygenic to Omnigenic. Cell; 169(7):1177–1186. pmid:28622505
  13. 13. Miska EA, Ferguson-Smith AC. Transgenerational inheritance: Models and mechanisms of non-DNA sequence-based inheritance. Science. 2016;354(6308): 59–63. pmid:27846492
  14. 14. Bohacek J, Mansuy IM. Molecular insights into transgenerational non-genetic inheritance of acquired behaviours. Nat Rev Genet. 2015 Nov;16(11):641–52. pmid:26416311
  15. 15. Sharma U, Sun F, Conine CC, Reichholf B, Kukreja S, Herzog VA, et al. Small RNAs Are Trafficked from the Epididymis to Developing Mammalian Sperm. Dev Cell. 2018 Aug 20;46(4):481–494.e6. pmid:30057273
  16. 16. Sharma A. Transgenerational epigenetics: Integrating soma to germline communication with gametic inheritance. Mech. Ageing Dev. 163, 15–22 (2017). pmid:28093237
  17. 17. Lumey LH. Decreased birthweights in infants after maternal in utero exposure to the Dutch famine of 1944–1945. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 1992;6:240–53. pmid:1584725
  18. 18. Bygren LO, Kaati G, Edvinsson S. Longevity determined by paternal ancestors’ nutrition during their slow growth period. Acta Biotheoret. 2001;49: 53–59.
  19. 19. Pembrey ME. Male-line transgenerational responses in humans. Hum Fertil (Camb). 2010 Dec;13(4):268–71. pmid:21117937
  20. 20. Pembrey ME, Bygren LO, Kaati G, Edvinsson S, Northstone K, Sjöström M, et al. Sex-specific, male-line transgenerational responses in humans. Eur J Hum Genet. 2006;14: 159–166. pmid:16391557
  21. 21. Northstone K, Golding J, Davey Smith G, Miller LL, Pembrey M. Prepubertal start of father’s smoking and increased body fat in his sons: further characterisation of paternal transgenerational responses. Eur J Hum Genet. 2014;22: 1382–1386. pmid:24690679
  22. 22. Golding J, Northstone K, Gregory S, Miller LL, Pembrey M. The anthropometry of children and adolescents may be influenced by the prenatal smoking habits of their grandmothers: A longitudinal cohort study. Am J Human Biol. 2014;26: 731–9 pmid:25130101
  23. 23. Zhao L, Kirkmeyer SV, Tepper BJ. A paper screening test to assess genetic taste sensitivity to 6-n-propylthiouracil. Physiology & behavior. 2003 Apr 1;78(4–5):625–33.
  24. 24. Elsabbagh M, Cohen H, Cohen M, Rosen S, Karmiloff-Smith A. Severity of hyperacusis predicts individual differences in speech perception in Williams Syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2011 Jun;55(6):563–71. pmid:21557785
  25. 25. Brendal MA, King KA, Zalewski CK, Finucane BM, Introne W, Brewer CC, Smith ACM. Auditory Phenotype of Smith-Magenis Syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2017 Apr 14;60(4):1076–1087. pmid:28384694
  26. 26. Rosenhall U, Nordin V, Sandström M, Ahlsen G, Gillberg C. Autism and hearing loss. J Autism Dev Disord. 1999;29(5): 349–357. pmid:10587881
  27. 27. Gomes E, Pedroso FS, Wagner MB. Auditory hypersensitivity in the autistic spectrum disorder. Pró-Fono. 2008;20(4): 279–284 pmid:19142473
  28. 28. Wilson US, Sadler KM, Hancock KE, Guinan JJ Jr, Lichtenhan JT. Efferent inhibition strength is a physiological correlate of hyperacusis in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Neurophysiol 118: 1164–1172, 2017. First published June 7, 2017; pmid:28592687
  29. 29. Zuko A, Kleijer KT, Oguro-Ando A, Kas MJ, van Daalen E, van der Zwaag B et al. Contactins in the neurobiology of autism. Eur J Pharmacol 2013; 719: 63–74. pmid:23872404
  30. 30. Mercati et al. CNTN6 mutations are risk factors for abnormal auditory sensory perception in autism spectrum disorders. Mol Psychiatry. 2017 Apr;22(4):625–633. pmid:27166760
  31. 31. Nilsson EE, Sadler-Riggleman I, Skinner MK. Environmentally induced epigenetic transgenerational inheritance of disease. Environ Epigenet. 2018;4(2): dvy016. pmid:30038800
  32. 32. Illum LRH, Bak ST, Lund S, Nielsen AL. DNA methylation in epigenetic inheritance of metabolic diseases through the male germ line. J Mol Endocrinol. 2018 Feb;60(2):R39–R56. pmid:29203518
  33. 33. Boucher BJ, Ewen SW, Stowers JM. Betel nut (Areca catechu) consumption and the induction of glucose intolerance in adult CD1 mice and in their F1 and F2 offspring. Diabetalogia 1994; 37: 49–55.
  34. 34. Drake AJ, Walker BR. The intergenerational effects of fetal programming: non-genomic mechanisms for the inheritance of low birth weight and cardiovascular risk. J Endocrinol 2004;180:1–16. pmid:14709139
  35. 35. Anderson LM, Riffle L, Wilson R, Travlos GS, Lubomirski MS, Alvord WG. Preconceptional fasting of fathers alters serum glucose in offspring of mice. Nutrition 2006;22:327–31. pmid:16500559
  36. 36. Dunn GA, Bale TL. Maternal high-fat diet effects on third-generation female body size via the paternal lineage. Endocrinology. 2011 Jun;152(6):2228–36. pmid:21447631
  37. 37. Saavedra-Rodríguez L, Feig LA. Chronic social instability induces anxiety and defective social interactions across generations. Biol Psychiatry. 2013 Jan 1;73(1):44–53. pmid:22906514
  38. 38. Champagne FA. Effects of stress across generations: why sex matters. Biol Psychiatry 2013;73:2–4. pmid:23217458
  39. 39. Gapp K, Jawaid A, Sarkies P, Bohacek J, Pelczar P, Prados J, et al. Implication of sperm RNAs in transgenerational inheritance of the effects of early trauma in mice. Nat Neurosci. 2014 May;17(5):667–9. pmid:24728267
  40. 40. Gapp K, Soldado-Magraner S, A40.lvarez-Sánchez M, Bohacek J, Vernaz G, Shu H, et al. Early life stress in fathers improves behavioural flexibility in their offspring. Nat Commun. 2014 Nov 18;5:5466. pmid:25405779
  41. 41. Manners MT, Yohn NL, Lahens NF, Grant GR, Bartolomei MS, Blendy JA. Transgenerational inheritance of chronic adolescent stress: Effects of stress response and the amygdala transcriptome. Genes Brain Behav. 2018 Jun 13:e12493.
  42. 42. Bak ST, Sakellariou D, Pena-Diaz J. The dual nature of mismatch repair as antimutator and mutator: for better or for worse. Front Genet. 2014 Aug 21;5:287. pmid:25191341
  43. 43. Van Baak TE, Coarfa C, Dugue PA, Fiorito G, Laritsky E, Baker MS, et al. Epigenetic supersimilarity of monozygotic twin pairs. Genome Biol. 2018; 19: 2. pmid:29310692
  44. 44. Pembrey M, Saffery R, Bygren LO. Network in epigenetic epidemiology. Human transgenerational responses to early-life experience: potential impact on development, health and biomedical research. J Med Genet. 2014;51: 563–572. pmid:25062846
  45. 45. Tobi EW, Lumey LH, Talens RP, Kremer D, Putter H, Stein AD, et al. DNA methylation differences after exposure to prenatal famine are common and timing- and sex-specific. Hum Mol Genet 2009; 18: 4046–4053. pmid:19656776
  46. 46. Kaati G, Bygren LO, Edvinsson S. Cardiovascular and diabetes mortality determined by nutrition during parents’ and grandparents’ slow growth period. Eur J Hum Genet. 2002; 10: 682–688. pmid:12404098
  47. 47. Vågerö D, Pinger PR, Aronsson V, van den Berg GJ. Paternal grandfather's access to food predicts all-cause and cancer mortality in grandsons. Nat Commun. 2018;9(1): 5124. pmid:30538239
  48. 48. Miller LL, Pembrey M, Davey Smith G, Northstone K, Golding J. Is the growth of the fetus of a non-smoking mother influenced by the smoking of either grandmother while pregnant? PLoS One 2014;9(2): e86781. pmid:24504157
  49. 49. Van den Berg GJ, Pinger PR. Transgenerational effects of childhood conditions on third generation health and education outcomes. Econ Hum Biol. 2016;23: 103–120. pmid:27592272
  50. 50. Lodge CJ, Bråbäck L, Lowe AJ, Dharmage SC, Olsson D, Forsberg B. Grandmaternal smoking increases asthma risk in grandchildren: a nationwide Swedish cohort. Clin Exp Allergy. 2018;48(2): 167–174. pmid:28925522
  51. 51. Yen AM, Boucher BJ, Chiu SY, Fann JC, Chen SL, Huang KC, et al. Longer duration and earlier age of onset of paternal betel chewing and smoking increase metabolic syndrome risk in human offspring, independently, in a community-based screening program in Taiwan. Circulation 2016;134: 392–404. pmid:27448815
  52. 52. Laubenthal J, Zlobinskaya O, Poterlowicz K, Baumgartner A, Gdula MR, Fthenou E, et al. Cigarette smoke-induced transgenerational alterations in genome stability in cord blood of human F1 offspring. FASEB J (2012) 26:3946–56. pmid:22730438
  53. 53. Mekhail K. Defining the damaged DNA mobility paradox as revealed by the study of telomeres, DSBs, microtubules and motors. Front Genet. 2018 Mar 20;9: 95. Erratum in: Front Genet. 2018;9: 157. pmid:29616083
  54. 54. Boyd A, Golding J, Macleod J, Lawlor DA, Fraser A, Henderson J, et al. Cohort Profile: The ‘Children of the 90s’; the index offspring of The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42: 111–127. pmid:22507743
  55. 55. Fraser A, Macdonald-Wallis C, Tilling K, Boyd A, Golding J, Davey Smith G, et al. Cohort Profile: The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42: 97–110. pmid:22507742
  56. 56. Golding J, ALSPAC Study Team. The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC)–study design and collaborative opportunities. Eur J Endocrinol 2004; 151(Suppl.3): U119–U123.
  57. 57. Birmingham K. Pioneering Ethics in a Longitudinal Study: The Early Development of the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee. Bristol: Public Policy Press, 2018.
  58. 58. UK Control of Noise at Work Regulations. 2005. Available from http://www.legislation.gov.uk/uksi/2005/1643/contents/made
  59. 59. Standard Occupational Classification 1990. London: Office of Population Censuses and Surveys; 1990.
  60. 60. Joris PX. Recruitment of neurons and loudness. Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology. 2009 Mar 1;10(1):1–4. pmid:19159978
  61. 61. Auerbach BD, Rodrigues PV, Salvi RJ. Central gain control in tinnitus and hyperacusis. Front Neurol. 2014;5: 206. pmid:25386157
  62. 62. Knipper M, Van Dijk P, Nunes I, Rüttiger L, Zimmermann U. Advances in the neurobiology of hearing disorders: recent developments regarding the basis of tinnitus and hyperacusis. Prog Neurobiol. 2013;111: 17–33. pmid:24012803
  63. 63. Khramtsova EA, Davis LK, Stranger BE. The role of sex in the genomics of human complex traits. Nat Rev Genet. 2018. pmid:30581192
  64. 64. Stringer S, Polderman TJC, Posthuma D. Majority of human traits do not show evidence for sex-specific genetic and environmental effects. Sci Rep. 2017;7(1): 8688. pmid:28819253