Figures
Abstract
The presentation of drug-associated cues has been shown to elicit craving and dopamine release in the striatum of drug-dependent individuals. Similarly, exposure to tobacco-associated cues induces craving and increases the propensity to relapse in tobacco- dependent smokers. However, whether exposure to tobacco-associated cues elicits dopamine release in the striatum of smokers remains to be investigated. We hypothesized that presentation of smoking-related cues compared to neutral cues would induce craving and elevation of intrasynaptic dopamine levels in subregions of the striatum and that the magnitude of dopamine release would be correlated with subjective levels of craving in briefly abstinent tobacco smokers. Eighteen participants underwent two [11C]-(+)-PHNO positron emission tomography (PET) scans after one-hour abstinence period: one during presentation of smoking-associated images and one during presentation of neutral images. Smoking cues significantly increased craving compared to neutral cues on one, but not all, craving measures; however, this increase in craving was not associated with overall significant differences in [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding potential (BPND) (an indirect measure of dopamine release) between the two experimental conditions in any of the brain regions of interest sampled. Our findings suggest that presentation of smoking cues does not elicit detectable (by PET) overall increases in dopamine in humans after one-hour nicotine abstinence. Future research should consider studying smoking cue-induced dopamine release at a longer abstinence period, since recent findings suggest the ability of smoking-related cues to induce craving is associated with a longer duration of smoking abstinence.
Citation: Chiuccariello L, Boileau I, Guranda M, Rusjan PM, Wilson AA, Zawertailo L, et al. (2013) Presentation of Smoking-Associated Cues Does Not Elicit Dopamine Release after One-Hour Smoking Abstinence: A [11C]-(+)-PHNO PET Study. PLoS ONE 8(3): e60382. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0060382
Editor: Wolfgang Blenau, Goethe University Frankfurt, Germany
Received: January 9, 2013; Accepted: February 25, 2013; Published: March 29, 2013
Copyright: © 2013 Chiuccariello et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Funding: This research received project support from the Canadian Tobacco Control Research Initiative (CTCRI) (http://ctcri.ca/en/, grant number 018035), the Pfizer GRAND Award (http://www.grand2012.org/, grant number WS508591) and salary support from and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) (http://www.cihr-irsc.gc.ca/e/193.html) Tobacco Use in Special Populations (TUSP). The funders had no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: Dr. Le Foll has received operating grant funding for other studies from Pfizer and is a consultant for Richter Pharmaceuticals, Lundbeck, Mylan, Ethypharm and Pfizer. Drs. Wilson and Houle have received operating grant funding for other studies from Eli Lilly, Lundbeck, and SK Life Science in the past 12 months. None of these companies participated in the design or execution of this study or writing the manuscript. This does not alter the authors' adherence to all the PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials.
Introduction
Smoking is the leading preventable cause of morbidity and mortality in Western Society. Although a majority of smokers express the desire to quit smoking, most are unsuccessful [1], [2]. Exposure to contextual-cues associated with drug-use induces craving [3], [4], [5], [6] and increases the propensity to relapse in drug users [7], [8], [9], [10]. Understanding the mechanism of cue-induced relapse is essential in developing evidence-based treatment strategies that could reduce the impact of cue-induced cravings in smokers.
Dopamine (DA) neurons, shown to respond to reward (drug or other) and to “conditioned” predictors of reward, are believed to be involved in response to drug-cues [11]. Specifically, DA neuron firing and DA release measured with microdialysis or voltametry have been associated with response to drug associated cues in most [12], [13], [14], [15], [16] though not all [17] animal studies. Positron Emission Tomography (PET) has allowed the investigation of the role of DA in response to conditioned cues in humans in vivo [6], [12], [18], [19], [20], [21], [22], [23].
PET is versatile and a minimally invasive technique that can be used to assess dopaminergic response to a pharmacological or non-pharmacological challenge (such as conditioned stimuli presentation) in humans [24]. In this regard, PET studies have shown DA release in sub-compartments of the striatum of both cocaine [6], [22] and opiate [21] addicted individuals in response to drug-related imagery, which was related to addiction severity and drug craving [6], [22]. fMRI as well as FDG-PET studies have echoed some of these findings by showing activation of reward associated regions during presentation of drug-related cues to drug addicted individuals [4], [10], [25], [26], [27], [28], [29], [30], [31], [32]. Despite the evidence from animal models of addiction, and neuroimaging studies suggesting the importance of DA in cue-induced drug-seeking and craving, no research to date has investigated the dopaminergic response to presentation of smoking-related cues compared to neutral cues in tobacco smokers.
In this study we used PET and the DA D2/3 agonist radioligand [11C]-(+)-PHNO [33], in combination with a validated smoking cue-paradigm [34], [35] to test the hypothesis that presentation of smoking-related images would elicit cravings and would increase DA release in the striatum and that the two phenomena would be related. Our choice of using [11C]-(+)-PHNO (vs. [11C]raclopride) was motivated by the finding that [11C]-(+)-PHNO has a higher displacement potential relative to [11C]raclopride and therefore may be more sensitive to detect acute fluctuations in DA release induced by smoking-related conditioned cues [36], [37], [38].
Materials and Methods
Ethics Statement
This study was approved by the Centre for Addiction and Mental Health (CAMH) Research Ethics Board in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Prior to participation in this study and following a detailed explanation of the study protocol, all participants provided written informed consent.
Study Participants
Participants between the ages of 18–45 were recruited by advertisement in local newspapers and on-line postings. They were non treatment-seeking regular smokers of ≥10 cigarettes per day, for at least the past two years and scored greater ≥4 on the Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND) [39]. Participants did not meet criteria for abuse or dependence on any other drug of abuse and tested negative at screening on a broad-spectrum gas chromatography and mass spectroscopy urine drug toxicology screen for drugs of abuse, including cannabis. Specific exclusion criteria included pregnancy, current medication use, claustrophobia, cardiovascular or cerebrovascular disease, any Axis I psychiatric disorder (other than nicotine dependence) as determined by the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) [40], a history of neurological illness/head trauma, learning disabilities, and the presence of metal objects in the body. Participants were not told to abstain from smoking but were asked not to consume caffeine on the day of the PET scan.
Experimental procedure
Subjects were invited to take part in a PET study, which took place on two separate days, at least one week apart. On each scan day, upon arrival at the CAMH lab, subjects were instructed to smoke one cigarette of their preferred brand one hour before the scan. Following this, expired carbon monoxide (CO) (Micro III Smokerlyzer, Bedfont Instruments, Kent, England) was measured. This interval was chosen to minimize withdrawal symptoms that may lead to a ceiling effect on self-reported craving and mask cue-induced subjective changes [41]. Subjects were also told that they should expect to smoke one hour after the scan to control for expectancy, which could affect DA release [30], [42], [43].
One hour after smoking, subjects were placed in a supine position inside the PET camera and fitted with the thermoplastic head fixation system to minimize movement (Orfit Industries, USA). Scans were performed using a PET/CT camera system (Siemens Medical Imaging, Knoxville TN), which measures radioactivity in 81 trans axial slices with a reconstructed pixel size of 1.07×1.07×2.0 mm each with an in-plane resolution of 5 mm full-width at half maximum (FWHM). An intravenous line was inserted in the antecubital vein for [11C]-(+)-PHNO injection (radiosynthesis described in [33]). A transmission scan was acquired and the emission scan, acquired in 32-bit list mode over 90 minutes, began after bolus injection of [11C]-(+)-PHNO (mean ± SD, dose: 10.02±0.69 mCi; specific activity: 1206.45±448.37 mCi/µmol; mass: 2.31±0.77 µg). Emission data were reconstructed by 2D filtered back projection to yield dynamic images with 15 one-minute frames and 15 five-minute frames.
The cue-paradigm took place when the subject was lying down inside the PET camera and consisted of viewing a picture slideshow through a set of goggles (Icuiti Corporation, Rochester New York). All subjects viewed two different slideshows on separate occasions, in a randomized and counter-balanced order: either smoking-associated pictures (images of individuals smoking or smoking paraphernalia) [34], [35] or neutral pictures (everyday images, such as a window or a face of someone not smoking) [44]. These cues were shown to effectively induce craving by others [34], [35], [44]. This specific cue paradigm was validated in a set of six smokers meeting the same inclusion and exclusion criteria prior to completing this study and shown to elicit craving in 1-hour abstinent smokers (unpublished). A tactile component was also added in this cue paradigm; during the presentation of the smoking-associated cues, subjects were asked to hold a cigarette, and during the presentation of the neutral cues, a pen. This has previously been shown to elicit a greater craving response in conjunction with a visual cue paradigm [45]. The cues were presented in four ten-minute blocks with five-minute break intervals between blocks with an approximate total time of forty minutes of cue presentation (out of the 95 minute scan time). Each block consisted of approximately forty-five pictures, with three pictures being shown per seven seconds each followed by a fixation cross for twenty-one seconds. Following the final block, the subjects were asked to remain still until the end of the scan. [11C]-(+)-PHNO was injected after the first 10-minute block of cue presentation. The choice of injection time and length of imagery presentation was based on the work of Volkow and colleagues [6].
Subjective levels of craving and withdrawal symptoms were assessed when subject were placed into the scanner, before cue presentation (Pre-cue) and at scan completion (Final). Craving measures included a 21-item nicotine-specific Visual Analog Scale (VAS; 100 point continuous scale to score from agree to disagree or not at all to very much), the abbreviated Questionnaire of Smoking Urges (QSU) [46], the Tobacco Craving Questionnaire (TCQ) [47] and the Minnesota Nicotine Withdrawal Scale (MNWS) [48]. The VAS question “I have a craving for a cigarette” was also assessed at each five-minute break during the cue paradigm (Block 1–4) and every 15 minutes (twice, Block 5–6) for the rest of the scan.
On a separate day all subjects completed a proton density weighted MRI, acquired on a 1.5 T Signa-GE Scanner (TE = 13, TR>5300 ms, FOV = 22×22, 256×256, slice thickness = 2 mm, NEX = 2) for the purpose of region of interest (ROI) delineation.
Data Analysis
Statistical Analysis. Differences in self-reported questionnaire measures and [11C]-(+)-PHNO regional binding between the two experimental conditions (neutral-cues vs. smoking-associated cues) were analyzed using repeated measures analysis of variance (RM-ANOVA). Paired sample t-tests were used for post-hoc comparisons. All analyses were two-tailed with an alpha level of 0.05. All tests were performed using the statistical software package SPSS, release 15.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago IL, USA). Change in DA levels between the two experimental conditions was calculated as a percentage difference in binding potential (BPND) obtained under neutral cues vs. smoking-associated cues. Correlations between change of DA and other outcome measures (such as expired CO and demographics) were calculated using Pearsons correlations. For measurement of craving during each experimental PET session, we measured the area under the curve (AUC) for the ratings obtained using the VAS question “I have a craving for a cigarette”. Absolute difference in AUC between the neutral and smoking cue conditions served as a single measure of cue-induced craving to be related to changes in BPND between the two experimental conditions.
PET Image Analysis.
All PET images were analyzed using the automated image analysis software Regions of Mental Interest (ROMI) (details in [49]).
Bilateral sub-compartments of the striatum, including sensorimotor striatum (SMST), associative striatum (AST), and limbic striatum (LST) were automatically segmented [49] as described in [50]. The (whole) Globus Pallidus (GP) was delineated with the procedure previously described and validated [51]. Identification of midbrain grey matter voxels within the substantia nigra (SN) region was performed by using the the previously described automated procedure [49]. Cerebellar cortex (excluding vermis, lobules IX and X) served as reference region and has been previously described [49]. [11C]-(+)-PHNO time activity curves were obtained from dynamic data, and specific BPND was estimated in each ROI using the simplified reference tissue method (SRTM) [52]. Parameter estimation was performed with PMOD (Version 2.8.5; PMOD Technologies Ltd, Zurich, Switzerland).
Results
Subject Demographics
Out of 24 subjects that were determined to be eligible for the study, 18 completed the study. Two participants had a reaction to [11C]-(+)-PHNO (nausea), 3 did not feel comfortable with the scanning procedures and 1 tested positive for a pregnancy screen on the day of the scan. None of the participants that were included in the study tested positive for drugs of abuse on a gas chromatography and mass spectroscopy broad-spectrum urine toxicology drug screen. Subject demographics appear in Table 1.
Craving and Withdrawal Measures
Subjective craving measures are illustrated in Figure 1. Repeated measures analysis of variance (RM-ANOVA) of craving scores obtained with the VAS question “I have a craving for a cigarette” indicated separate main effects of cue-type (F(1,17) = 6.935, p<0.05) and time (F(7,119) = 18.098, p<0.005) on self reported craving such that self-reported craving was greater during presentation of smoking vs. neutral cues and self reported craving increased with time (pre cue vs. block 2–6 all p<0.05). Time since last cigarette also significantly increased self-reported urge to smoke for pleasurable effects (QSU Factor 1 F(1,17) = 53.866, p<0.001; TCQ 2/expectancy F(1,17) = 25.510, p<0.001), urge to smoke for relief of negative withdrawal effects (QSU factor 2 F(1,17) = 27.591, p<0.001; TCQ 1/emotionality F(1,17) = 11.907, p<0.005,) intention to smoke (TCQ 4/purposefulness F(1,17) = 14.336, p<0.005) and withdrawal symptoms (MNWS F(1,17) = 23.996, p<0.001); however these measures were not different between conditions (all p>0.05). Exposure to smoking-related compared to neutral cues did not significantly increase self-reported withdrawal symptoms (MNWS), urge to smoke for pleasurable effects (QSU Factor 1 or TCQ 2), relief of negative effects (QSU Factor 2 or TCQ 1) or intention to smoke (TCQ 4).
(a) Visual analog scale measurements of craving for a cigarette assessed every fifteen minutes during cue-presentation (Blocks 1–4) and every 15 minutes following the completion of the paradigm (Blocks 5–6). There was a main effect of cue type (Repeated Measures Analysis of Variance (RM-ANOVA, F(1,17) = 6.935, p<0.05) and time (RM-ANOVA, F(7,119) = 18.098, p<0.005) on craving for a cigarette. (b) Withdrawal symptoms as rated by the Minnesota Nicotine Withdrawal Scale at baseline (prior to cue presentation) and at the end of the scan. There was a main effect of time on withdrawal symptoms (RM-ANOVA, F(1,17) = 23.996, p<0.001) (c) Cigarette craving as measured by the Questionnaire of Smoking Urges Factor 1 (urge to smoke for pleasure effects of smoking) and Factor 2 (urge to smoke for relief of negative effect of withdrawal) at baseline and at the end of the scan. There was a main effect of time on the urge to smoke for pleasurable effects (RM-ANOVA, QSU Factor 1 F(1,17) = 53.866, p<0.001) and on the urge to smoke for relief of negative symptoms (RM-ANOVA, QSU Factor 2, F(1,17) = 27.591, p <0.001) (d) Cigarette craving measured by the Tobacco Craving Questionnaire factors 1 (Emotionality), 2 (Expectancy), 3 (Compulsivity) and 4 (Purposefulness) at baseline and at the end of the scan. There was a main effect of time on the urge to smoke for relief of negative withdrawal effects (RM-ANOVA, TCQ 1/emotionality F(1,17) = 11.907, p<0.005), urge to smoke for pleasurable effects (RM-ANOVA, TCQ 2/expectancy F(1,17) = 25.510, p<0.001) and intention to smoke (RM-ANOVA, TCQ 4/purposefulness F(1,17) = 14.336, p<0.005). * p<0.05, ** p<0.01, *** p<0.005
PET Findings
RM-ANCOVA controlling for age and expired CO averaged over both conditions revealed no significant main effect of condition (i.e.: smoking vs neutral cues) (F(1,15) = 0.053 p = 0.821) and interaction with regions of interest (AST, LST, SMST, GP and SN) (F(4, 60) = 2.09, p = 0.99) (Figure 2). Overall [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND during presentation of smoking cues was ∼6% greater when compared to presentation of neutral cues (Figure 2). There were no significant correlations between cue-induced craving (AUC using the repeated VAS measures) and % change in ROI binding from the neutral condition. There were also no correlations between any of the subjective self-report variables, demographic information (i.e.: severity of addiction), expired CO and regional cue-induced changes in binding.
[11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND in the associative striatum (AST), limbic striatum (LST), sensorimotor striatum (SMST), globus pallidus (GP) and substantia nigra (SN) in the neutral and smoking cue conditions. There were no overall significant differences between the cue conditions in [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND in any of the apriori selected regions of interest.
As a post-hoc analysis, a comparison of [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND in high cravers (VAS AUC greater than sample mean, n = 8) vs. low cravers (VAS AUC lower than sample mean, n = 8) revealed no significant differences in percent change [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND in any of the regions of interest (multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) F(5,10) = 0.759, p = 0.731) (Figure 3). Two participants were not included in this analysis because they were within one standard deviation of the sample mean.
(a) Area under the Visual Analog Scale curve in high cravers and low cravers. (b) Percent change in [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND in the associative striatum (AST), limbic striatum (LST), sensorimotor striatum (SMST), globus pallidus (GP) and substantia nigra (SN) in high cravers versus low cravers. There were no significant differences in percent change in [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND in any of the aprior regions of interest when comparing high cravers to low cravers (multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) F(5,10) = 0.759, p = 0.731).
Discussion
To our knowledge, this is the first study to assess fluctuation of DA levels in response to presentation of smoking-related cues compared to presentation of neutral cues in human smokers using PET. We found that presentation of smoking cues in our setting elicited craving on the VAS, but this finding was not supported by other measures of craving (QSU, TCQ). We also found that smoking cue presentation after a short (one hour) abstinence period did not result in changes in DA release as measured by changes in [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND.
In this cue paradigm, ratings of smoking-cue induced craving and of withdrawal increased over time in both experimental conditions. The VAS scale question “I have a craving for a cigarette”, which was repeatedly assessed throughout the cue presentation, revealed a significant increase in craving in the smoking-associated cue condition relative to the neutral condition (See Fig. 1A). However, we found that on most measures of craving, the increase was similar under both experimental conditions (See Fig. 1 B,C,D). Several factors may have impacted our ability to detect significant increases in craving on the QSU and TCQ. First, it is possible that the experimental conditions (subjects being placed in a PET scanner and pictures provided through goggles) may have created an unusual setting which was not conducive to natural situations in which cues may significantly induce craving for cigarettes. We did show in a pilot study that our smoking cues were capable of inducing craving relative to the neutral cues on the VAS in a sample of six participants (unpublished data) and other investigators have been able to induce craving with similar paradigms for a variety of drugs of abuse [6], [22], [25], [30], [31] including tobacco cigarettes [25], [28], [31]. A second possibility for the lack of difference between the two conditions on craving on the QSU and TCQ is that the plasma half-life of nicotine is on average 2 hours [53], and it is likely that there was still detectable levels of nicotine (in the plasma and also likely in the brain) of the participants during the sessions, minimizing craving and potentially the ability to detect cue-induce DA release. Thirdly, the duration of abstinence that we chose may have not been the optimal time point to capture changes in craving as assessed by the QSU and TCQ. Recent research has investigated the effect of abstinence time on smoking cue-induced craving and it was determined that smoking cue-induced craving increases with duration of abstinence [3]. Specifically, when comparing groups asked to remain abstinent for different periods of time (7 days, 14 days, 35 days) smoking cue-induced craving was greatest at 35 days of abstinence, suggesting an incubation of cue-induced craving [3]. It is of interest to note that in that study the subjects also did not display cue-induced craving after a short abstinence period.
Contrary to the research done in human studies of cocaine and opioid dependence [6], [21], [22], we were not able to detect significant differences in binding potential between the two conditions. A possible explanation could be related to the lack of difference we saw between conditions on craving scores as measured by the QSU and TCQ. Previous studies performed in cocaine users indicated that while analyzing the results based on the cravings scores, only the group displaying robust cravings scores had an associated elevation of DA levels in the striatal area [22]. However, we saw that even those participants that were the strongest cravers, as measured by the area under the VAS curve, did not show a significant percent change in [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND. Another possibility that could explain the lack of change in DA level in our paradigm could be related to the expectancy conditions that were chosen. DA neurons have been shown to respond to reward prediction error and under some conditions (stimuli presentation not resulting in the occurrence of the predicted reward) there may be decreases in DA cell firing [20]. Such phenomenon could account for the lack of elevation of DA, as measured by in vivo microdialysis, that has been reported under some conditions, such as contingent presentation of cocaine-associated stimuli in non-human primates[17]. The knowledge of occurrence of reward (e.g. possibility of smoking a cigarette) immediately following cue presentation could lead to strong activation of the reward pathway [25], [54] and it appears that glucose metabolism and activation (as measured by fMRI) in regions associated with reward is greatest when individuals are expecting to receive drug versus no expectation of drug [30], [54]. Collectively, this research suggests that the expectation of drug can influence the neurobiological reaction to drug or drug-associated cues. As participants in the current study were told that they would be able to smoke only one hour after exposure to the cue paradigm, this delay of accessing the reward after the cue presentation session may have contributed to an inability of our conditions to elicit elevation of DA levels. Another possibility for the lack of significant difference in [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND between conditions is the fact that as subjects were likely under the influence of some nicotine, their dopaminergic system may have been already stimulated by nicotine and may have prevented the cue-induced craving to produce any further activation. This could mean that nicotine-induced DA release could have masked any cue-induced DA release. It is interesting to note that presence or absence of nicotine did not affect cue-elicited craving [55] or brain activation [43] in some studies, suggesting that some cravings or brain responses can be detected while nicotine is on board (however, see [25]).
We do not believe that the inability to detect an elevation of DA levels in our experiment is related to the choice of radiotracer. Although [11C]raclopride is perhaps the gold standard for measuring binding at D2/3 receptors, it is thought that [11C]-(+)-PHNO may be superior to measure acute fluctuations in synaptic DA release [36], [37]. When comparing DA-induced displacement of [11C]-(+)-PHNO and [11C]raclopride binding through a d-amphetamine challenge in anesthetized cats, there was approximately 83% inhibition of [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding potential compared to only approximately 56% of [11C]raclopride binding potential [37]. In a more recent direct comparison of ligands, DA release in the dorsal striatum, induced by an amphetamine challenge, was found to cause a 1.5 time greater reduction in [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding compared to [11C]raclopride binding [36]. However, [11C]-(+)-PHNO is not without limitations. Due to the sensitivity of [11C]-(+)-PHNO to detect small changes in DA release, it is also possible that it may have been more sensitive to the DA releasing effect of nicotine that may have been present after the short abstinence period. In addition, [11C]-(+)-PHNO also binds to D3 receptors, which can provide up to 100% of the binding signal in certain brain regions (substantia nigra), and [11C]-(+)-PHNO may therefore have a different sensitivity to detect DA release in these regions [56]. The current research did not find any differences in DA release between the two conditions (smoking vs. neutral), nor was there a greater magnitude of change in the D3 –enriched regions. [11C]-(+)-PHNO does act as an agonist at DA D2 and D3 receptors and previous studies have reported that pharmacological effects are present in approximately 14.3% of subjects scanned using this radiotracer [57]. Furthermore, it has been suggested that many studies utilizing [11C]-(+)-PHNO PET may use non-tracer doses [58]. Non-tracer doses may present a bias or underestimation of [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND, and therefore the mass dose effect should be taken in to consideration [36]. In the present study, although there was some variability in the specific activity at the time of injection within in each condition, we found no correlation between specific activity and [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND in either of the conditions across any of the ROIs (p>0.05). Since we do not see a significant difference in the mass injected between the smoking and neutral cue conditions, we do not believe that the mass of radiotracer contributed to the negative findings of the current research. Finally, the test-retest reliability of [11C]-(+)-PHNO in the striatum ranges from approximately 2–7% [38]. We do not believe that the reason for an overall lack of main effect of condition on DA release was due to a lack of power to detect a difference (effect size = 0.45).
This study showed no overall significant difference in DA levels between tobacco cue and neutral cue presentation conditions when measured at one-hour abstinence in human tobacco smokers. One limitation of this study is that under this short abstinence period, increases in craving were not detectable by the QSU and TCQ but there were significant increases on the repeated VAS throughout the cue paradigm. Further studies would need to explore the ability of tobacco cue presentation to elicit cravings and associated DA release after longer period of abstinence.
Acknowledgments
We thank technicians Alvina Ng and Laura Nguyen, chemistry staff Armando Garcia, Winston Stableford and Min Wong, and engineers Terry Bell and Ted Harris-Brandts, for their assistance with this project. Dr. Le Foll takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis, and that all authors had full access to all the data in the study.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: LC BLF LZ UB AW SH IB. Performed the experiments: LC MG. Analyzed the data: LC IB PR. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: AW SH. Wrote the paper: LC IB BLF.
References
- 1. Hughes JR (1992) Tobacco withdrawal in self-quitters. J Consult Clin Psychol 60: 689–697.
- 2. Lindstrom M (2001) Desire to stop smoking among intermittent and daily smokers: a population-based study. Tob Control 10: 396–397.
- 3. Bedi G, Preston KL, Epstein DH, Heishman SJ, Marrone GF, et al. (2011) Incubation of cue-induced cigarette craving during abstinence in human smokers. Biol Psychiatry 69: 708–711.
- 4. Due DL, Huettel SA, Hall WG, Rubin DC (2002) Activation in mesolimbic and visuospatial neural circuits elicited by smoking cues: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Am J Psychiatry 159: 954–960.
- 5. Erblich J, Bovbjerg DH (2004) In vivo versus imaginal smoking cue exposures: is seeing believing? Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 12: 208–215.
- 6. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, et al. (2006) Cocaine cues and dopamine in dorsal striatum: mechanism of craving in cocaine addiction. J Neurosci 26: 6583–6588.
- 7. Abrams DB, Monti PM, Carey KB, Pinto RP, Jacobus SI (1988) Reactivity to smoking cues and relapse: two studies of discriminant validity. Behav Res Ther 26: 225–233.
- 8. Niaura R, Abrams D, Demuth B, Pinto R, Monti P (1989) Responses to smoking-related stimuli and early relapse to smoking. Addict Behav 14: 419–428.
- 9. Janes AC, Pizzagalli DA, Richardt S, de BFB, Chuzi S, et al. (2010) Brain reactivity to smoking cues prior to smoking cessation predicts ability to maintain tobacco abstinence. Biol Psychiatry 67: 722–729.
- 10. Sinha R, Li CS (2007) Imaging stress- and cue-induced drug and alcohol craving: association with relapse and clinical implications. Drug Alcohol Rev 26: 25–31.
- 11. Schultz W (1998) Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. J Neurophysiol 80: 1–27.
- 12. Ito R, Dalley JW, Howes SR, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ (2000) Dissociation in conditioned dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens core and shell in response to cocaine cues and during cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. J Neurosci 20: 7489–7495.
- 13. Katner SN, Kerr TM, Weiss F (1996) Ethanol anticipation enhances dopamine efflux in the nucleus accumbens of alcohol-preferring (P) but not Wistar rats. Behav Pharmacol 7: 669–674.
- 14. Katner SN, Weiss F (1999) Ethanol-associated olfactory stimuli reinstate ethanol-seeking behavior after extinction and modify extracellular dopamine levels in the nucleus accumbens. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 23: 1751–1760.
- 15. Phillips PE, Stuber GD, Heien ML, Wightman RM, Carelli RM (2003) Subsecond dopamine release promotes cocaine seeking. Nature 422: 614–618.
- 16. Stuber GD, Roitman MF, Phillips PE, Carelli RM, Wightman RM (2005) Rapid dopamine signaling in the nucleus accumbens during contingent and noncontingent cocaine administration. Neuropsychopharmacology 30: 853–863.
- 17. Bradberry CW, Barrett-Larimore RL, Jatlow P, Rubino SR (2000) Impact of self-administered cocaine and cocaine cues on extracellular dopamine in mesolimbic and sensorimotor striatum in rhesus monkeys. J Neurosci 20: 3874–3883.
- 18. Melendez RI, Rodd-Henricks ZA, Engleman EA, Li TK, McBride WJ, et al. (2002) Microdialysis of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens of alcohol-preferring (P) rats during anticipation and operant self-administration of ethanol. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 26: 318–325.
- 19. Schiffer WK, Liebling CN, Reiszel C, Hooker JM, Brodie JD, et al. (2009) Cue-induced dopamine release predicts cocaine preference: positron emission tomography studies in freely moving rodents. J Neurosci 29: 6176–6185.
- 20. Schultz W (1997) Dopamine neurons and their role in reward mechanisms. Curr Opin Neurobiol 7: 191–197.
- 21. Zijlstra F, Booij J, van den Brink W, Franken IH (2008) Striatal dopamine D2 receptor binding and dopamine release during cue-elicited craving in recently abstinent opiate-dependent males. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 18: 262–270.
- 22. Wong DF, Kuwabara H, Schretlen DJ, Bonson KR, Zhou Y, et al. (2006) Increased occupancy of dopamine receptors in human striatum during cue-elicited cocaine craving. Neuropsychopharmacology 31: 2716–2727.
- 23. Boileau I, Dagher A, Leyton M, Welfeld K, Booij L, et al. (2007) Conditioned dopamine release in humans: a positron emission tomography [11C]raclopride study with amphetamine. J Neurosci 27: 3998–4003.
- 24. Laruelle M (2000) Imaging synaptic neurotransmission with in vivo binding competition techniques: a critical review. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 20: 423–451.
- 25. McClernon FJ, Kozink RV, Lutz AM, Rose JE (2009) 24-h smoking abstinence potentiates fMRI-BOLD activation to smoking cues in cerebral cortex and dorsal striatum. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 204: 25–35.
- 26. McClernon FJ, Kozink RV, Rose JE (2008) Individual differences in nicotine dependence, withdrawal symptoms, and sex predict transient fMRI-BOLD responses to smoking cues. Neuropsychopharmacology 33: 2148–2157.
- 27. Kosten TR, Scanley BE, Tucker KA, Oliveto A, Prince C, et al. (2006) Cue-induced brain activity changes and relapse in cocaine-dependent patients. Neuropsychopharmacology 31: 644–650.
- 28. Smolka MN, Buhler M, Klein S, Zimmermann U, Mann K, et al. (2006) Severity of nicotine dependence modulates cue-induced brain activity in regions involved in motor preparation and imagery. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 184: 577–588.
- 29. Vollstadt-Klein S, Kobiella A, Buhler M, Graf C, Fehr C, et al. (2011) Severity of dependence modulates smokers' neuronal cue reactivity and cigarette craving elicited by tobacco advertisement. Addict Biol 16: 166–175.
- 30. McBride D, Barrett SP, Kelly JT, Aw A, Dagher A (2006) Effects of expectancy and abstinence on the neural response to smoking cues in cigarette smokers: an fMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology 31: 2728–2738.
- 31. Franklin TR, Wang Z, Wang J, Sciortino N, Harper D, et al. (2007) Limbic activation to cigarette smoking cues independent of nicotine withdrawal: a perfusion fMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology 32: 2301–2309.
- 32. Brody AL, Mandelkern MA, London ED, Childress AR, Lee GS, et al. (2002) Brain metabolic changes during cigarette craving. Arch Gen Psychiatry 59: 1162–1172.
- 33. Wilson AA, McCormick P, Kapur S, Willeit M, Garcia A, et al. (2005) Radiosynthesis and evaluation of [11C]-(+)-4-propyl-3,4,4a,5,6,10b-hexahydro-2H-naphtho[1,2-b][1,4]oxazin-9-ol as a potential radiotracer for in vivo imaging of the dopamine D2 high-affinity state with positron emission tomography. J Med Chem 48: 4153–4160.
- 34. Hussain S, Zawertailo L, Busto U, Zack M, Farvolden P, et al. (2010) The impact of chronic bupropion on plasma cotinine and on the subjective effects of ad lib smoking: a randomized controlled trial in unmotivated smokers. Addict Behav 35: 164–167.
- 35. van Hanswijck de Jonge L, Gormley M (2005) Responses to positive and negative smoking-related images: effects of current smoking status and degree of smoking addiction. Addict Behav 30: 1587–1591.
- 36. Shotbolt P, Tziortzi AC, Searle GE, Colasanti A, van der Aart J, et al. (2012) Within-subject comparison of [(11)C]-(+)-PHNO and [(11)C]raclopride sensitivity to acute amphetamine challenge in healthy humans. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 32: 127–136.
- 37. Ginovart N, Galineau L, Willeit M, Mizrahi R, Bloomfield PM, et al. (2006) Binding characteristics and sensitivity to endogenous dopamine of [11C]-(+)-PHNO, a new agonist radiotracer for imaging the high-affinity state of D2 receptors in vivo using positron emission tomography. J Neurochem 97: 1089–1103.
- 38. Willeit M, Ginovart N, Graff A, Rusjan P, Vitcu I, et al. (2008) First human evidence of d-amphetamine induced displacement of a D2/3 agonist radioligand: A [11C]-(+)-PHNO positron emission tomography study. Neuropsychopharmacology 33: 279–289.
- 39. Heatherton TF, Kozlowski LT, Frecker RC, Fagerstrom KO (1991) The Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence: a revision of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire. Br J Addict 86: 1119–1127.
- 40. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, et al. (1998) The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry 59 Suppl 20: 22–33;quiz 34-57.
- 41. Hendricks PS, Ditre JW, Drobes DJ, Brandon TH (2006) The early time course of smoking withdrawal effects. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 187: 385–396.
- 42. Dols M, van den Hout M, Kindt M, Willems B (2002) The urge to smoke depends on the expectation of smoking. Addiction 97: 87–93.
- 43. McClernon FJ, Hiott FB, Huettel SA, Rose JE (2005) Abstinence-induced changes in self-report craving correlate with event-related FMRI responses to smoking cues. Neuropsychopharmacology 30: 1940–1947.
- 44.
Lang PJaBBP (1997) International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Technical Manual and Affective Ratings. Attention.
- 45. Niaura R, Shadel WG, Abrams DB, Monti PM, Rohsenow DJ, et al. (1998) Individual differences in cue reactivity among smokers trying to quit: effects of gender and cue type. Addict Behav 23: 209–224.
- 46. Tiffany ST, Drobes DJ (1991) The development and initial validation of a questionnaire on smoking urges. Br J Addict 86: 1467–1476.
- 47. Heishman SJ, Singleton EG, Moolchan ET (2003) Tobacco Craving Questionnaire: reliability and validity of a new multifactorial instrument. Nicotine Tob Res 5: 645–654.
- 48. Hughes JR, Hatsukami D (1986) Signs and symptoms of tobacco withdrawal. Arch Gen Psychiatry 43: 289–294.
- 49. Rusjan P, Mamo D, Ginovart N, Hussey D, Vitcu I, et al. (2006) An automated method for the extraction of regional data from PET images. Psychiatry Res 147: 79–89.
- 50. Martinez D, Slifstein M, Broft A, Mawlawi O, Hwang DR, et al. (2003) Imaging human mesolimbic dopamine transmission with positron emission tomography. Part II: amphetamine-induced dopamine release in the functional subdivisions of the striatum. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 23: 285.
- 51. Rusjan P (2008) Kinetics of [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding in the globus pallidus. Neuroimage 41: T137.
- 52. Lammertsma AA, Hume SP (1996) Simplified reference tissue model for PET receptor studies. Neuroimage 4: 153.
- 53. Hukkanen J, Jacob P 3rd, Benowitz NL (2005) Metabolism and disposition kinetics of nicotine. Pharmacol Rev 57: 79–115.
- 54. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Ma Y, Fowler JS, Zhu W, et al. (2003) Expectation enhances the regional brain metabolic and the reinforcing effects of stimulants in cocaine abusers. J Neurosci 23: 11461–11468.
- 55. Tiffany ST, Cox LS, Elash CA (2000) Effects of transdermal nicotine patches on abstinence-induced and cue-elicited craving in cigarette smokers. J Consult Clin Psychol 68: 233–240.
- 56. Tziortzi AC, Searle GE, Tzimopoulou S, Salinas C, Beaver JD, et al. (2010) Imaging dopamine receptors in humans with [11C]-(+)-PHNO: dissection of D3 signal and anatomy. Neuroimage 54: 264–277.
- 57. Mizrahi R, Houle S, Vitcu I, Ng A, Wilson AA (2010) Side effects profile in humans of (11)C-(+)-PHNO, a dopamine D(2/3) agonist ligand for PET. J Nucl Med 51: 496–497.
- 58. Searle GE, Beaver JD, Tziortzi A, Comley RA, Bani M, et al. (2012) Mathematical modelling of [(11)C]-(+)-PHNO human competition studies. Neuroimage 68: 119–132.