Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

The German validation of the Body Perception Questionnaire-Short Form (BPQ-SF) and its relation to current self-report measures of interoception

  • Sebastian Brand ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Sebastian Brand, Markus Roman Tünte, Carlos Ventura-Bort

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy of Childhood and Adolescence, Johannes Gutenberg University, Mainz, Germany

  • Markus Roman Tünte ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Sebastian Brand, Markus Roman Tünte, Carlos Ventura-Bort

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna, Wien, Austria, Vienna Doctoral School Cognition, Behavior and Neuroscience, University of Vienna, Wien, Austria

  • Michael Witthöft,

    Roles Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Clinical Psychology, Psychotherapy and Experimental Psychopathology, Johannes Gutenberg University, Mainz, Germany

  • Stefanie Hoehl,

    Roles Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna, Wien, Austria

  • Mathias Weymar,

    Roles Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Biological Psychology and Affective Science, Faculty of Human Sciences, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany, Faculty of Health Sciences Brandenburg, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany

  • Carlos Ventura-Bort

    Contributed equally to this work with: Sebastian Brand, Markus Roman Tünte, Carlos Ventura-Bort

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    ventura@uni-potsdam.de

    Affiliation Department of Biological Psychology and Affective Science, Faculty of Human Sciences, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany

Abstract

The Body Perception Questionnaire-Short Form (BPQ-SF) is one of the most used questionnaires to assess interoception. Although the BPQ-SF has been translated into different languages, there is no validated German questionnaire adaptation so far. Furthermore, empirical evidence outlining how the BPQ-SF relates to novel theories of interoception that distinguish between different facets, such as attention and accuracy, is still missing. The current study therefore aims at (1) validating the German version of BPQ-SF (with data from three different sites, Ntotal = 1292) and (2) relating it to the constructs of interoceptive accuracy and attention. In line with the original validation of the BPQ-SF, in the German version, an adequate three-factor structure and good internal consistency were found. However, reliability indexes varied between good, for the supra-and subdiaphragmatic scales and poor, for the body awareness scale. The three scales of the BPQ-SF showed significant associations with interoceptive measures and, more importantly, were related to both interoceptive accuracy and attention, suggesting that they mirror a mixture of both constructs. Finally, in relation to measures of psychopathology, the supra-and subdiaphragmatic (but not the body awareness) scales were positively related to alexythimic, anxious, and depressive tendencies. These findings provide evidence for the usability of the German version of the BPQ-SF and further shed light on the heterogeneity of the existing self-report measures of interoception.

Introduction

The ability to perceive and process internal bodily signals, such as heart rate or gastrointestinal changes, is defined as interoception [1]. Interoceptive abilities have been related to various psychological processes and clinical tendencies (e.g., [112]). They are typically classified based on the nature of the interoceptive measure: behavioral, self-report, or their correspondence [4]. One of the most used self-report measures to evaluate interoception is the Body Perception Questionnaire-Short Form (BPQ-SF; [13, 14]). Since its introduction, the BPQ-SF has been widely used and has contributed to extensive literature on how interoception influences human behavior and experience [4, 15]. New approaches to conceptualizing and defining interoception highlight the need to distinguish between different facets of interoceptive processing within its domain (e.g., behavioral, self-report) [12, 16]. However, empirical evidence outlining how the BPQ-SF relates to these novel theories of interoceptive processing is still missing. Further, although the English version was introduced in 1993 [13], the psychometric validation of the German version of the BPQ-SF has not yet been carried out yet. Tackling these gaps in the literature, the current study aims to validate the German BPQ-SF and situate it within a recent theoretical framework of interoceptive processing [12].

In light of the novel theoretical perspectives that call for a nuanced taxonomy of interoception beyond the nature of the measurement [1, 12], recent research has aimed at shedding light on the relationship between different measures of interoception in the behavioral [17] and self-report domain [15]. In a recent study, Desmedt and colleagues [15] first systematically reviewed the existing literature to identify the five most used self-report measurements of interoception which were the BPQ-SF, the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA; [18, 19]), the Body Awareness Questionnaire (BAQ; [20]), the Private subscale of the Body Consciousness Questionnaire (PBCS; [21]), and the Self-Awareness Questionnaire (SAQ; [22]). After that, these five questionnaires’ overall factorial and network structure was examined in a large sample. Results showed low convergence between questionnaires, indicating that these self-report measures tap into different constructs of interoception.

These results follow recent proposals emphasizing the distinction between different interoceptive constructs within the self-report (and behavioral) domain [12]. One such proposal is the 2 x 2 factorial model of interoception, which distinguishes between interoceptive accuracy, understood as correctly perceiving the actual state of one’s body, and interoceptive attention, defined as the degree to which a person attends or focuses on bodily changes. The distinction between self-reported interoceptive accuracy (assessed with the Interoceptive Accuracy Scale, IAS; [23]) and attention (measured with the Interoceptive Attention Scale, IATS; [24]) has been confirmed in recent studies showing that these two constructs are differentially associated with clinical outcomes, including anxiety, depression, alexithymia or somatoform symptomatology [25, 26]. Interestingly, the most used questionnaires of interoception, as identified by Desmedt et al. [15], were created before the new proposals of interoception emerged.

Considering the importance of providing validated measures and the novel theoretical proposals of interoception, the current study aimed to validate the German version of the BPQ-SF and assess its fit within the 2 x 2 factorial model [12]. Furthermore, the relationship between the BPQ-SF subscales and psychopathological tendencies such as anxiety, depression and alexithymia was examined. The BPQ-SF comprises three subscales conceptualized based on the organization of the autonomic neural pathways according to the polyvagal theory [2729]. The first scale, the Body Awareness scale, describes the sensibility to bodily signals arising from the afferent pathways. The other two, the Supradiaphragmatic and Subdiaphragmatic scales, assess the perceived reactivity from the ventral and dorsal vagal complex, which regulate organs above (e.g., face, head and visceral organs such as armpits) and below (e.g., visceral organs related to the urinary or digestive systems) the diaphragm. In the original English and subsequent Spanish versions, the BPQ-SF scales showed good internal consistency, and each scale showed an adequate model fit for a one-factor solution. Since its development, the BPQ-SF has successfully been cross-culturally validated [3032], but only recently the relationship between the 2x2 factorial model of interoception and the BPQ-SF subscales have been examined, mostly focusing on the body-awareness scale [e.g., 24]. These studies have generally found a positive association between the body awareness scale scores and the IATS scores, but the association with IAS scores has varied across studies [24, 30].

Following previous studies, we expected to find three different factors according to each subscale with at least an acceptable model fit, good internal consistency, and reliability indexes. In line with prior studies [23, 24, 30], we further expected that the BPQ-SF scales would show convergent validity with self-report measures of interoception. Furthermore, to explore the extent to which the subscales of the BPQ-SF are related to the constructs of self-reported interoceptive attention and accuracy, canonical correlational analysis (CCA) was performed. CCA allows for testing the association between sets of variables rather than looking at single correlations. Although single correlations inform about the relationship between variables, separately, they may provide an oversimplified understanding of the complex relation between the existing constructs [33]. Avoiding such simplification is particularly relevant in interoception research as it can contribute to a more comprehensive and global interpretation of the relationship between different interoception measures.

Method

Transparency and openness

This project emerged as a part of a broader multi-site collaboration [25, 26]. Data collection from most sites (https://aspredicted.org/K2L_8TW) and data analysis for the current project were preregistered (https://aspredicted.org/HC1_D39). Data is available via OSF server (https://osf.io/6s2qf/). Data for the current project was used in other publications [25, 26, 34].

Participants

Site 1.

400 German-speaking individuals recruited via the online platform prolific (https://www.prolific.co/) took part in the study (data collected between 1st July, 2021 and 1st July, 2022). Participants were monetarily compensated for participation (approximately 3.50€). If participants did not report a high proficiency in German, were below 18 or above 70 years old, or responded too fast or slow (SoSciSurvey variables DEG_TIME >100 or TIME_RSI >2; [35]), they were excluded from the analysis. The final sample comprised 388 participants (216 women, 166 men, 6 non-binary).

Site 2.

The sample consisted of 523 students who underwent a battery of questionnaires administered via Sona Systems (https://www.sona-systems.com/; data collected between November, 15th, 2020 and February 6th, 2022). Participants who did not report a high proficiency in German, were below 18 or above 70 years old or completed the survey double as fast as the sample mean time of completion (N = 43) were excluded from the analysis [35], resulting in a final sample of 480 participants (341 women, 78 men, 1 non-binary).

Site 3.

The initial sample consisted of 506 individuals recruited via social media from the German general population (data collected from March 21st, 2020 to June 21st, 2020). Participants who did not report a high proficiency in German or were below 18 or above 70 years of age were excluded, resulting in a total sample of N = 484 (349 women, 130 men, 5 non-binary).

Slightly different batteries of questionnaires were administered to each site (see Ns for each questionnaire in the Questionnaires subsection). The local ethics committees granted each site’s Data collection approval.

Ethic statement

Data collection was approved by the Ethics Committee of each psychological institute and/or university. Participants signed and informed consent prior to the participation of the study.

Questionnaires

Body Perception Questionnaire-Short Form (BPQ-SF) and Very Short Form (BPQ-VSF).

The BPQ-SF is composed of 46 items scored on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from (1) never to (5) always, grouped in three subscales, targeting two main domains: Body Awareness and autonomous nervous system reactivity (ANSR; [13, 14]). The Body Awareness subscale (26 items) assesses the proportion of time a person reports being aware of bodily sensations. The ANSR domain is measured with the supra- (15 items) and subdiaphragmatic (6 items) reactivity subscales. The supradiaphragmatic subscale quantifies the body reactions above the diaphragm, and the subdiaphragmatic scale measures the gastrointestinal functions below the diaphragm. One item (’I feel like vomiting’) is shared among both ANSR subscales.

As an alternative, a shorter measure of the body awareness domain was tested by Cabrera and colleagues [14]: the Body Perception Questionnaire-Very Short Form (BPQ-VSF). The BPQ-VSF comprises 12 items from the body awareness subscale of the BPQ-SF. The authors observed that the BPQ-VSF was a reliable measure of body awareness (see also [32]). In the current study, the BPQ-SF was administered to Site 1 and Site 2 samples, whereas Site 3 sample used only the BPQ-VSF. Therefore, Site 1 and Site 2 were used as validation samples for the BPQ-SF and BPQ-VSF, whereas Site 3 was used only as a replication sample for the BPQ-VSF. Of note, some of the items of the BPQ-VSF administered to the Site 3 sample had a slightly alternative linguistic construction (but identical meaning) to those used in the BPQ-SF (e.g., Völlegefühl oder Aufgeblähtsein was formulated as voller oder geblähter Magen). In the Tables A and B in S2 Text the German version of the BPQ-SF is provided.

Interoceptive Accuracy Scale (IAS).

The IAS was created to measure interoceptive accuracy within the 2 x 2 factorial model of interoception [23, 25]. The questionnaire consists of 21 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale. In the current study, the IAS showed good internal consistency (validation sample: ω = .88; replication sample, ω = .85. All participants completed the IAS, n = 1292.

Interoceptive Attention Scale (IATS).

Within the 2 x 2 factorial model of interoception [12], the IATS was designed to evaluate self-reported attention to interoceptive perceptions such as hunger or breathing [24]. The questionnaire compromises 21 items on a 5-point Likert scale. In the current study, the IATS showed good internal consistency, ω = .90. A total of n = 642 participants completed the IATS.

Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness Version-2 (MAIA-2).

The MAIA-2 [19, 36] focuses on the evaluation of multiple dimensions of interoception throughout its 37 items divided into 8 scales. The 8 subscales are Noticing (4 items), Not-Distracting (6 items), Not-Worrying (5 items), Attention Regulation (7 items), Emotional Awareness (5 items), Self-Regulation (4 items), Body Listening (3 items), and Trust (3 items). Each item is rated on a 6-point Likert scale. Overall, in the current study, the subscales of the MAIA-2 showed acceptable to good internal consistency: (validation/replication): ωNoticing = .72/.62, ωNonDistracting = .85/.82, ωNotWorrying = .80/.78, ωAttentionRegulation = .83/.82, ωEmotionalAwareness = .82/.80, ωSelfRegulation = .84/.84, ωBodyListening = .84/.76, and ωTrust = .86/.79. ldiation sample: ωNoticing = .70, ωNonDistracting = .83, ωNotWorrying = .78, ωAttentionRegulation = .82, ωEmotionalAwareness = .82, ωSelfRegulation = .84, ωBodyListening = .82, and ωTrust = .83. The MAIA-2 was completed by n = 1098 participants.

Interoceptive Confusion Questionnaire (ICQ)

The ICQ [37] consists of 20 items, evaluating the difficulties interpreting one’s non-affective physiological states, such as hunger, muscle pain, or arousal (e.g., ’I often find that I’m suddenly very thirsty’). The ICQ is scored on a 5-point Likert scale. In our samples, the internal consistency of the ICQ was acceptable, ω = .66. A total of 226 participants completed the ICQ.

Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20).

Alexithymia was assessed with the German version [38] of the TAS-20 [39], which consists of 20 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale grouped into three subscales: Difficulty Identifying Feelings (7 items), Difficulties Describing Feelings (5 items) and Externally Oriented Thinking (8 items). In the current study, the TAS-20, completed by 614 participants, showed good internal consistency, ω = .85.

Patient Health Questionnaire 15-item version (PHQ-15) and the Patient Health Questionnaire 9-item version (PHQ-9).

Somatic symptoms were measured using the PHQ-15 [40], and depressive symptoms were assessed using the PHQ-9 [41]. Both questionnaires are part of a German screening procedure for assessing psychological complaints in individuals (PHQ-D; [42]). The PHQ-15 consists of 15 items assessing the degree of individual somatic symptoms (e.g., abdominal pain) on a 3-point scale. The PHQ-9 consists of 9 items measuring the degree of individual distress caused by depressive symptoms. The respondents indicate to which extent they are burdened by symptoms such as sadness or hopelessness on a 4-point scale. The PHQ-15 and PHQ-9 showed good internal consistency, PHQ-15: ω = .78, PHQ-9: ω = .88. The PHQ-15 and PHQ-9 were completed by n = 484 participants.

The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II).

The BDI-II [43, 44] measures the severity of depressive symptoms. It consists of 21 statements assessing the presence of psychological (e.g., feelings of guilt) and physiological (e.g., loss of energy) symptoms of major depression. Statements are assigned point values reflecting the severity of depressive symptoms. The BDI-II showed good internal consistency in the current sample, ω = .91. Overall, n = 226 participants completed the BDI-II.

Anxiety Sensitivity Inventory 3 (ASI-3).

The Anxiety Sensitivity Inventory-3 (ASI-3; [45, 46]) measures anxiety sensitivity, a construct referring to a person’s fear of their physiological anxiety-related arousal response. The ASI-3 consists of 18 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale. In the current sample, the ASI-3 also showed good internal consistency, ω = .88. Overall, n = 226 participants completed the ASI-3.

State-Trait-Anxiety Inventory (STAI).

The STAI [47] intends to measure both state (S) and trait anxiety (T). In the current study, only the trait subscale was used. This subscale consists of 20 items rated on a 4-point Likert scale. The STAI-T showed good internal consistency in the present samples, ω = .93. Overall, n = 226 participants completed the STAI-T.

Statistical analysis

The software IBM SPSS Statistics [48], Mplus [49], and R version 4.0.5 [50] were used to perform the statistical analyses. Within R, we used the packages tydiverse, psych, lavaan, lme4, cca, and cocor [5156].

Preliminary analyses were conducted to test for differences between Site 1 and Site 2 samples in BPQ-SF subscale and BPQ-VSF total scores. No differences between samples were observed in any of the subscales of the BPQ-SF (Bonferroni-corrected p-value = .017, all p’s > .03, Cohen’s d < .14). Although, for the BPQ-VSF, differences between samples arose (Site 1 vs Site 2, t776.68 = 2.67, p = .007, d = 0.1), these were rather small (Msite1 = 35.9 and Msite2 = 37.7), indicating that both samples had comparable BPQ-related scores and could be collapsed. Therefore, the demographic characteristics, convergent validity, and internal consistency were tested separately for the validation (Site 1 and Site 2 samples collapsed) and replication samples. Similarly, the factor structure of the BPQ-SF subscales (only validation sample) and the BPQ-SVF (validation and replication samples) were performed for each sample, separately.

Confirmatory analysis of the structure of the Body Perception Questionnaire—(Very) Short Form (BPQ[V]-SF).

Following the validation methodology outlined by Cabrera and colleagues [14], the scores of items in the BPQ-(V)SF were dichotomized, with responses indicating no assignment receiving a score of zero and all other responses receiving a score of one. A predetermined three-factor solution based on established knowledge [14] was applied to the BPQ-SF. This solution comprised factors related to body awareness (items 1–26), supradiaphragmatic reactivity (items 27–41), and subdiaphragmatic reactivity (items 41 to 46). Similarly, for the BPQ-VSF, a one-factor solution (utilizing 12 items from the BPQ-SF) was fitted independently for both the validation and replication samples. Confirmatory factor analysis was executed employing the robust mean and variance-adjusted weighted least squares (WLSMV) procedure, which is recommended for models dealing with discrete data [57]. χ 2-tests were performed to evaluate the model fit for the confirmatory factor solution. Recognizing the χ2-value’s sensitivity to influence from large sample sizes, supplementary metrics, including the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), and the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) were also considered following previous research [58, 59].

Descriptive characteristics.

Descriptive characteristics of the BPQ-SF, including mean, standard deviation, skewness, and kurtosis, are reported in the Table A in S1 Text. Correlational analysis was performed to investigate the association between age and the BPQ-SF subscales and the BPQ-VSF scores. Gender differences in BPQ-SF subscales and BPQ-VSF scores were evaluated using unpaired t-tests.

Internal consistency and test-retest reliability.

Internal consistency was computed through the application of McDonald’s omega. To assess test-retest reliability, a subset of participants from the Potsdam sample (n = 57) underwent retesting. Participants were allowed to freely sign up for a retest after completing the online session without an initial time limit between the initial and retest sessions. However, only participants who completed the retests within 200 days after the initial session were considered for analysis.

Test-retest reliability was calculated using Pearson’s and Spearman’s correlation coefficients and the Intraclass Correlation Coefficient (ICC). Given the considerable variability in the number of days between the test and retest sessions (up to 200 days), an examination was conducted to determine whether the number of days between these sessions moderated the test-retest relationship. This analysis involved multiple regressions, with the scores at time 1 (i.e., test) as the criterion, and the scores at time 2 (i.e., retest), the time elapsed between time 1 and time 2 (in days), and their interaction as predictors.

Convergent validity with interoceptive-related and other questionnaires.

Convergent validity was examined by testing the relationship between the BPQ-SF subscales and BPQ-VSF with other interoception scales (MAIA-2 subscales, IAS, IATS, ICQ) as well as with psychopathology-related scales (TAS-20, PHQ9, PHQ15, STAI-T, ASI-3, and BDI-II). Convergent validity was assessed using Pearson’s correlations.

Canonical correlation analysis.

To investigate how the BPQ-SF scales relate to different facets of interoception proposed in the 2x2 factorial model of interoception, we focused on the distinction between interoceptive accuracy and attention [12]. We conducted a canonical correlation analysis in R using the CCA package [56], with one factor comprising the IAS and IATS, and the other was composed of the BPQ-SF subscales using the collapsed Site 1 and Site 2 samples.

Results

Demographic data

Age was not related to the Body Awareness subscale (r = -.01, p = .660), but a significant, negative correlation was found with the Supradiaphragmatic subscale (r = -.11, p = .002). The subdiaphragmatic scale was not significantly related to age (r = .01, p = .84), whereas the BPQ-VSF showed a positive relationship with age in the replication but not in the validation sample, validation sample: r = -.01, p = .803, replication sample: r = .16, p < .001.

To test for gender differences in each subscale, participants who self-identified as non-binary were excluded from the analysis due to the small group size (n = 12). Although gender differences did not reach significance (Bonferroni-corrected p-value = .016) for the Body Awareness subscale, t451.53 = 2.02, p = .044, d = 0.15 (Mwomen = 82.7, Mmen = 79.8), significant differences emerged in the supradiaphragmatic, t468.24 = 3.11, p = .002, d = 0.24 (Mwomen = 23.6, Mmen = 22.0), and subdiaphragmatic subscales, t551.53 = 5.51, p < .001, d = 0.40 (Mwomen = 11.2, Mmen = 9.7), indicating higher scores for women than men. For the BPQ-VSF, gender differences were found in the validation sample, t452.7 = 2.04, p = .041, d = 0.16 (Mwomen = 37 .3, Mmen = 35 .8) but not in the replication sample, t222.66 = 0.01, p = .99, d = 0.01 (Mwomen = 39.7, Mmen = 39.7).

Confirmatory factor analysis

In line with established knowledge regarding potential factor solutions, we employed confirmatory factor analysis to apply a three-factor model to the Body Perception Questionnaire-Short Form (BPQ-SF) and a one-factor model to the Body Perception Questionnaire-Very Short Form (BPQ-VSF) for validation and replication samples separately. The corresponding fit indices for each analysis are presented in Table 1. According to the criteria outlined by Schermelleh-Engel and colleagues [59], all solutions demonstrated a good fit, as evidenced by the RMSEA. The CFI and TLI were noted to approach the acceptable threshold of .95 in the three-factor solution for the BPQ-SF. The BPQ-VSF one-factorial solutions signified an acceptable fit (> .95). Furthermore, in the short versions, we observed robust modelling of the factor structure in the replication sample, as evidenced by a nonsignificant χ2-test. The factor loadings for each version of the questionnaire and sample are detailed in Table 2.

thumbnail
Table 2. Standardised factor loadings from confirmatory factor analysis for the BPQ-SF and BPQ-VSF.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmen.0000038.t002

Internal consistency and test-retest reliability

The Body Awareness subscale showed excellent internal consistency, ω = .93, and the Supra-, ω = .87, and Subdiaphragmatic subscales showed good internal consistency, ω = .82. Similar internal consistency was observed for the BPQ-VSF (validation sample: ω = .87, replication sample: ω = .87).

A total of n = 57 participants completed the BPQ-SF a second time to test for test-retest reliability. The Body Awareness scale of the BPQ-SF showed poor test-retest reliability (r = .47, rs = .45, ICC = .46). Whereas the supradiaphragmatic and subdiaphragmatic subscales showed good test-retest reliability (Supradiaphragmatic: r = .79, rs = .73, ICC = .79; Subdiaphragmatic: r = .77, rs = .71, ICC = .77). Like the Body Awareness scale, the BPQ-VSF showed poor test-retest reliability (r = .53, rs = .52, ICC = .52).

Multiple regressions were performed to test the effects of time passed on the relation between test and retest scores. For the Body Awareness subscale of the BPQ-SF, the scores at time 2 significantly predicted the scores at time 1, β = 0.69, t = 3.72, p < .001, 95% CI [0.32, 1.07], but no evidence for an effect of days passed, β = 0.32, t = 1.93, p = .058 95% CI [−0.01, 0.66], or interaction was found, β = −0.01, t = -1.77, p = .080, 95% CI [−0.01, 0.01]. Similar results were observed for the Supradiaphragmatic subscale, as indicated by a significant effect of scores at time 2, β = 0.78, t = 7.53, p < .001, 95% CI [0.57, 0.99]. Still, no evidence for the effects of days passed, β = 0.04, t = 0.34, p = .734, 95% CI [−0.07, 0.10], or the interaction, β = -0.01, t = -0.82, p = .420, 95% CI [−0.01, 0.01]. However, for the Subdiaphragmatic subscale, along with a significant effect of scores at time 2, β = 0.97, t = 7.61, p < .001, 95% CI [0.71, 1.22], significant effects of days passed, β = 0.05, t = 2.4, p = .020, 95% CI [0.01, 0.12], as well as interaction effects were found, β = -0.01, t = -2.04 p = .047, 95% CI [-0.01,- 0.01].

Finally, for the BPQ-VSF, a significant effect of scores at time 2 was observed, β = 0.92, t = 4.65, p < .001, 95% CI [0.52, 1.32], as well as significant effects of days passed, β = 0.19, t = 2.19, p = .030, 95% CI [0.02, 0.35], and days passed x score at time 2 interaction effects, β = -0.01, t = -2.14 p = .038, 95% CI [-0.01,- 0.01].

Convergent validity with interoception-related questionnaires

The complete list of correlations between BPQ-SF subscales, BPQ-VSF, and other measurements is depicted in Table 3. To test the significance of the relationship, a Bonferroni correction was applied (p = .05/13 questionnaires = .003).

thumbnail
Table 3. Correlational analysis between BPQ-SF subscales, BPQ-VSF, and other measurements.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmen.0000038.t003

Associations with interoceptive accuracy (IAS).

Body Awareness scores were positively related to IAS scores r = .308, p < .001, 95% CI [.255, .361]. A similar relationship was observed with the BPQ-VSF validation sample, r = .306, p < .001, 95% CI [.253, .358], and replication sample, r = .439, p < .001, 95% CI [.376, .497]. In contrast, Supradiaphragmatic scores were negatively related to self-reported interoceptive accuracy, r = -.210, p < .001, 95% CI [-.260, -.150].

Associations with interoceptive attention (IATS).

Subjective interoceptive attention scores were positively related to all subscales of the BPQ and the BPQ-VSF. Body Awareness: r = .198, p < .001, 95% CI [.135, .26], supradiaphragmatic: r = .320, p < .001, 95% CI [.265, .382], subdiaphragmatic: r = .330, p < .001, 95% CI [.275, .391], BPQ-VSF, r = .195, p < .001, 95% CI [.132, .257].

Associations with the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness Version-2 (MAIA-2) subscales.

The Body Awareness subscale was positively associated with the Attention Regulation, r = .160, p < .001, 95% CI [.093, .223], Emotional Awareness, r = .260, p < .001, 95% CI [.200, .325], and Self-Regulation scale, r = .170, p < .001, 95% CI [.110, .24,], as well as Body Listening scale, r = .140, p < .001, 95% CI [.070, .200].

A similar pattern was observed for the BPQ-VSF. In the validation sample, positive relationships with the Attention Regulation, r = .185, p < .001, 95% CI [.119, .248], Emotional Awareness, r = .230, p < .001, 95% CI [.230, .353], and Self-Regulation scale, r = .190, p < .001, 95% CI [.128, .256], as well as Body Listening scale, r = .149, p < .001, 95% CI [.083, .214], were observed. This pattern was also found in the replication sample, for Attention Regulation, r = .280, p < .001, 95% CI [.213, .352], Emotional Awareness, r = .350, p < .001, 95% CI [.280, .412], and Self-Regulation scales, r = .181, p < .001, 95% CI [.108, .253], as well as Body Listening scales, r = .256, p < .001, 95% CI [.185, .325].

On the other hand, the Supra- and Subdiaphragmatic scales showed negative associations with the Not Worrying (supradiaphragmatic: r = -.210, p < .001, 95% CI [-.277, -.149], subdiaphragmatic: r = -.154, p < .001 (95% CI [-.220, -.089]), Attention Regulation (supradiaphragmatic: r = -.210, p < .001, 95% CI [-.268, -.140], subdiaphragmatic: r = -.130, p < .001, 95% CI [-.199, -.067]), and Trust scales (supradiaphragmatic: r = -.250, p < .001, 95% CI [-.310, -.190], subdiaphragmatic: r = -.210, p < .001, 95% CI [-.268, -.141]).

Interoceptive Confusion Questionnaire (ICQ).

The ICQ showed negative relationships with Body Awareness, r = -.180, p < .001, 95% CI [-.291, -.078], and BPQ-VSF, r = -.210, p < .001, 95% CI [-.309, -.098], but was positively correlated to the Supradiaphragmatic scale, r = .320, p < .001, 95% CI [.218, .416].

Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20 (TAS-20).

The overall scores of the TAS-20 were positively related to supra-, r = .300, p < .001, 95% CI [.233 –.356], and subdiaphragmatic scale scores, r = .160, p < .001, 95% CI [.091, .220].

Psychopathological symptoms.

Scores in the BDI-II were positively related to supra-, r = .360, p < .001, 95% CI [.260, .452], and subdiaphragmatic scale scores, r = .250, p < .001, 95% CI [.139, .35]. Similarly, both scales were positively related to the STAI-T, supradiaphragmatic, r = .350, p < .001, 95% CI [.249, .443], subdiaphragmatic, r = .230, p < .001, 95% CI [.118, .327], and ASI-3 scores, supradiaphragmatic, r = .380, p < .001, 95% CI [.277, .466], subdiaphragmatic, r = .270, p < .001, 95% CI [.164, .368].

Canonical correlation analysis.

Canonical Correlation Analysis revealed significant relationships between both factors. Two canonical variates were extracted; a canonical variate is a composite variable created from a weighted combination of the original variables within each set, such that the correlation between these composites from each set is maximized. We found that both canonical variates showed a significant canonical correlation: first canonical correlation: 0.407 (Wilks’ λ = 0.703, Rao’s F(6, 1274) = 40.98, p < .001), second canonical correlation: 0.397 (Wilks’ λ = 0.842, Rao’s F(2, 638) = 59.70, p < .001).

Next, we investigated the canonical loadings. Table 4 displays the canonical loadings for each variable within the canonical variates. One canonical variate loaded strongly on the IAS, indicating it captures variance related to an interoceptive accuracy dimension, while the second canonical variate loaded strongly on the IATS, suggesting that it captures variance associated with an interoceptive attention dimension. The BPQ-SF subscales had significant loadings across both dimensions, indicating that they tap into both constructs captured by the canonical variates (see also Fig 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Canonical variates.

Note: IAS = Interoceptive Accuracy Scale; IATS = Interoceptive Attention Scale; BPQ_AWARE = Body Perception Questionnaire Awareness Subscale; BPQ_SUB = Subdiaphragmatic Scale; BPQ_SUPRA = Supdiaphragmatic Subscale.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmen.0000038.g001

Discussion

The current study aimed to validate the German version of the BPQ-(V)SF, one of the most used interoception questionnaires [15] and examine its fit within new models of interception. In line with the initial [14] and subsequent adaptation to other languages [3032], in the German version of the BPQ-SF, the three-factor solution showed a good model fit. The three subscales further revealed good to excellent internal consistency. As predicted, subscales of the BPQ-SF were significantly associated with other measures of interoception, but such relationships varied across scales. Furthermore, CCA indicated that the subscales of the BPQ-SF do not specifically relate to interoceptive accuracy or attention but load on both facets.

Confirmatory factor analysis revealed the three-factor solution to be a good fit for the German version of the BPQ-SF. These results align with previous validations of the BPQ-SF [3032] and further support the theoretical conceptualization of the questionnaire. In addition, the BPQ-VSF also demonstrated an excellent one-factor solution, replicating previous findings [14, 31]. It should be noted that, as for other versions of the BPQ-SF, in the German version not all items loaded optimally to the three factors according to current guidelines [60], suggesting that the questionnaire may benefit from further refinement. Although all scales showed good to excellent internal consistency, the good test-retest reliability observed for the Supra- and Subdiaphragmatic scales contrasted with the poor reliability scores observed for the Body Awareness scale and the BPQ-VSF. These findings suggest that self-reported autonomic reactivity might reflect more stable constructs than self-reported awareness of bodily changes, which may be more influenced by short-term, fluctuating factors [26].

In relation to demographic characteristics, a significant negative relationship between age and the Supra- and Subdiaphragmatic scales was observed. These results align with previous validations of the BPQ-SF [14, 31] and concur with findings suggesting decreased sensibility of autonomic reactivity with age [61]. We also observed lower autonomic reactivity scores in men than women, partly replicating previous findings [31]. Although no specific hypotheses were derived in this regard, gender differences in performance tasks [62, 63] and self-reported measures of interoception have often [26, 31] but not always [25] been found. However, these findings have pointed to both increased [26, 62] and decreased interoceptive processing for women compared to men [63]. Our results thus add to the mixed results on the relation between gender and interoception.

As expected, scales of the BPQ-(V)SF showed significant associations with other self-report measures of interoception. However, the direction and significance of these associations varied across scales. Whereas the Body Awareness subscale and the BPQ-SF showed positive, albeit small, associations with most of the interoceptive scales, the Supra- and Subdiaphragmatic subscales showed nonsignificant or negative associations with interoceptive questionnaire scales (except for a positive association with the IATS). To shed more light on how the BPQ-SF subscales fit the recently proposed 2 x 2 factorial model of interoception [12], CCA was performed, using self-reported interoceptive accuracy and attention as canonical variates. Results from the CCA indicate that the BPQ-SF subscales are multidimensional, loading on interoceptive accuracy and attention factors. These results contradict prior studies that assumed that the Body Awareness subscale reflects self-reported interoceptive attention (e.g., [30]). Instead, the Body Awareness scales may mirror a mixture of both constructs. Our results are thus in line with previous findings [15], highlighting the lack of consistency between the existing self-report measures of interoception and ratifying the need for more comprehensive frameworks that encompass the different interoceptive constructs measured with the current self-report measures [1, 15, 16].

In relation to questionnaires of psychopathological symptomatology, the BPQ-VSF and Body Awareness scale showed nonsignificant or negative associations. In contrast, the Supra and Subdiaphragmatic scales were positively related to alexithymic, anxious, and depressive tendencies. These findings are partly in line with previous results showing a positive association between autonomic reactivity and depression scores [32]. In a similar vein, Kolacz and colleagues [64] have recently observed a positive association between the autonomic reactivity scales of the BPQ-SF and disrupted physiological reactivity, including reduced heart rate variability, a psychophysiological readout typically associated with psychopathological vulnerability [65, 66]. Recent proposals have emphasized the role of disrupted autonomic functioning in clinical disorders (e.g., [67]). The current findings may thus provide indirect evidence for these proposals, demonstrating the negative role of higher autonomic reactivity on psychopathological tendencies.

Two main implications can be extracted from our results. First, improving our understanding of the ways in which existing self-report measures of interoception are related can help to clarify the taxonomy of interoception in a more comprehensive and inclusive manner. Second, highlighting the associations between different facets of interoception (e.g., autonomic reactivity) and psychopathological tendencies, may promote the development of intervention programs [68] that tackle specific interoceptive abilities to improve mental health.

Conclusion

In summary, the current findings provide evidence for the usability of the German version of the BPQ-(V)SF, extending previous questionnaire versions. By examining the relationship with other interoceptive and psychopathology-related questionnaires, the current study further shed light on the heterogeneity of the existing self-report measures of interoception and their differential relationship to psychopathology tendencies [26, 69], supporting the need for more comprehensive interoceptive frameworks [16].

Acknowledgments

We thank P.B. for her valuable contribution to the Site 3 sample data collection as part of her master thesis. We thank J. P. W. for his valuable contribution in the German translation of the BPQ-SF.

References

  1. 1. Khalsa SS, Adolphs R, Cameron OG, Critchley HD, Davenport PW, Feinstein JS et al. Interoception and Mental Health: A Roadmap. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 2018; 3(6):501–13. pmid:29884281
  2. 2. Jenkinson PM, Taylor L, Laws KR. Self-reported interoceptive deficits in eating disorders: A meta-analysis of studies using the eating disorder inventory. J Psychosom Res 2018; 110:38–45. pmid:29764604
  3. 3. Herbert BM, Herbert C, Pollatos O. On the relationship between interoceptive awareness and alexithymia: is interoceptive awareness related to emotional awareness? J Pers 2011; 79(5):1149–75. pmid:21241306
  4. 4. Garfinkel SN, Seth AK, Barrett AB, Suzuki K, Critchley HD. Knowing your own heart: distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biol Psychol 2015; 104:65–74. pmid:25451381
  5. 5. Garfinkel SN, Manassei MF, Hamilton-Fletcher G, den Bosch Y in, Critchley HD, Engels M. Interoceptive dimensions across cardiac and respiratory axes. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2016; 371(1708). pmid:28080971
  6. 6. Ehlers A. Interoception and panic disorder. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy 1993; 15(1):3–21.
  7. 7. Dunn BD, Galton HC, Morgan R, Evans D, Oliver C, Meyer M et al. Listening to your heart. How interoception shapes emotion experience and intuitive decision making. Psychol Sci 2010; 21(12):1835–44. pmid:21106893
  8. 8. Brewer R, Murphy J, Bird G. Atypical interoception as a common risk factor for psychopathology: A review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2021; 130:470–508. pmid:34358578
  9. 9. Witthöft M, Bräscher A-K, Jungmann SM, Köteles F. Somatic Symptom Perception and Interoception. Zeitschrift für Psychologie 2020; 228(2):100–9.
  10. 10. Quadt L, Garfinkel SN, Mulcahy JS, Larsson DE, Silva M, Jones A-M et al. Interoceptive training to target anxiety in autistic adults (ADIE): A single-center, superiority randomized controlled trial. EClinicalMedicine 2021; 39:101042. pmid:34401684
  11. 11. Paulus MP, Stewart JL. Interoception and drug addiction. Neuropharmacology 2014; 76 Pt B(0 0):342–50. pmid:23855999
  12. 12. Murphy J, Catmur C, Bird G. Classifying individual differences in interoception: Implications for the measurement of interoceptive awareness. Psychon Bull Rev 2019; 26(5):1467–71. pmid:31270764
  13. 13. Porges SW. Body perception questionnaire. Laboratory of Developmental Assessment: University of Maryland; 1993.
  14. 14. Cabrera A, Kolacz J, Pailhez G, Bulbena-Cabre A, Bulbena A, Porges SW. Assessing body awareness and autonomic reactivity: Factor structure and psychometric properties of the Body Perception Questionnaire-Short Form (BPQ-SF). Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 2018; 27(2):e1596. pmid:29193423
  15. 15. Desmedt O, Heeren A, Corneille O, Luminet O. What do measures of self-report interoception measure? Insights from a systematic review, latent factor analysis, and network approach. Biol Psychol 2022; 169:108289. pmid:35150768
  16. 16. Suksasilp C, Garfinkel SN. Towards a comprehensive assessment of interoception in a multi-dimensional framework. Biol Psychol 2022; 168:108262. pmid:35026353
  17. 17. Hickman L, Seyedsalehi A, Cook JL, Bird G, Murphy J. The relationship between heartbeat counting and heartbeat discrimination: A meta-analysis. Biol Psychol 2020; 156:107949. pmid:32911018
  18. 18. Mehling WE, Acree M, Stewart A, Silas J, Jones A. The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness, Version 2 (MAIA-2). PLoS One 2018; 13(12):e0208034. pmid:30513087
  19. 19. Mehling WE, Price C, Daubenmier JJ, Acree M, Bartmess E, Stewart A. The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA). PLoS One 2012; 7(11):e48230. pmid:23133619
  20. 20. Shields SA, Mallory ME, Simon A. The Body Awareness Questionnaire: Reliability and Validity. Journal of Personality Assessment 1989; 53(4):802–15.
  21. 21. Miller LC, Murphy R, Buss AH. Consciousness of body: Private and public. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1981; 41(2):397–406.
  22. 22. Longarzo M, D’Olimpio F, Chiavazzo A, Santangelo G, Trojano L, Grossi D. The relationships between interoception and alexithymic trait. The Self-Awareness Questionnaire in healthy subjects. Front Psychol 2015; 6:1149. pmid:26300829
  23. 23. Murphy J, Brewer R, Plans D, Khalsa SS, Catmur C, Bird G. Testing the independence of self-reported interoceptive accuracy and attention. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2020; 73(1):115–33. pmid:31519137
  24. 24. Gabriele E, Spooner R, Brewer R, Murphy J. Dissociations between self-reported interoceptive accuracy and attention: Evidence from the Interoceptive Attention Scale. Biol Psychol 2022; 168:108243. pmid:34929353
  25. 25. Brand S, Meis AC, Tünte MR, Murphy J, Woller JP, Jungmann SM et al. A multi-site German validation of the Interoceptive Accuracy Scale and its relation to psychopathological symptom burden. Commun Psychol 2023; 1(1).
  26. 26. Tünte MR, Petzke T, Brand S, Murphy J, Witthöft M, Hoehl S et al. He Who Seeks Finds (Bodily Signals): Differential Effects of Self-Reported Interoceptive Attention and Accuracy on Subclinical Psychopathology in a German-Speaking Sample; 2022.
  27. 27. Porges SW, Furman SA. The Early Development of the Autonomic Nervous System Provides a Neural Platform for Social Behavior: A Polyvagal Perspective. Infant Child Dev 2011; 20(1):106–18. pmid:21516219
  28. 28. Porges SW. Orienting in a defensive world: mammalian modifications of our evolutionary heritage. A Polyvagal Theory. Psychophysiology 1995; 32(4):301–18.
  29. 29. Porges SW. The polyvagal theory: new insights into adaptive reactions of the autonomic nervous system. Cleve Clin J Med 2009; 76 Suppl 2(Suppl 2):S86–90. pmid:19376991
  30. 30. Campos C, Rocha NB, Barbosa F. Untangling Self-Reported Interoceptive Attention and Accuracy: Evidence from the European Portuguese Validation of the Body Perception Questionnaire and the Interoceptive Accuracy Scale; 2021.
  31. 31. Cerritelli F, Galli M, Consorti G, D’Alessandro G, Kolacz J, Porges SW. Cross-cultural adaptation and psychometric properties of the Italian version of the Body Perception Questionnaire. PLoS One 2021; 16(5):e0251838. pmid:34043660
  32. 32. Wang N, Ren F, Zhou X. Factor Structure and Psychometric Properties of the Body Perception Questionnaire-Short Form (BPQ-SF) Among Chinese College Students. Front Psychol 2020; 11:1355. pmid:32714241
  33. 33. Sherry A, Henson RK. Conducting and interpreting canonical correlation analysis in personality research: a user-friendly primer. Journal of Personality Assessment 2005; 84(1):37–48. pmid:15639766
  34. 34. Ventura-Bort C, Wendt J, Weymar M. The Role of Interoceptive Sensibility and Emotional Conceptualization for the Experience of Emotions. Front Psychol 2021; 12:712418. pmid:34867591
  35. 35. Leiner DJ. Too Fast, too Straight, too Weird: Non-Reactive Indicators for Meaningless Data in Internet Surveys [229–248 Pages / Survey Research Methods, Vol 13 No 3 (2019)] 2019.
  36. 36. Eggart M, Todd J, Valdés-Stauber J. Validation of the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA-2) questionnaire in hospitalized patients with major depressive disorder. PLoS One 2021; 16(6):e0253913. pmid:34170963
  37. 37. Brewer R, Cook R, Bird G. Alexithymia: a general deficit of interoception. R Soc Open Sci 2016; 3(10):150664. pmid:27853532
  38. 38. Bach M, Bach D, de Zwaan M, Serim M. Validation of the German version of the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale in normal adults and psychiatric inpatients. Psychotherapie Psychosomatik Medizinische Psychologie 1996; 46(1):23–8.
  39. 39. Bagby RM, Taylor GJ, Parker JD. The Twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale—II. Convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity. J Psychosom Res 1994; 38(1):33–40. pmid:8126688
  40. 40. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW. The PHQ-15: validity of a new measure for evaluating the severity of somatic symptoms. Psychosom Med 2002; 64(2):258–66. pmid:11914441
  41. 41. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW. The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Intern Med 2001; 16(9):606–13. pmid:11556941
  42. 42. Gräfe K, Zipfel S, Herzog W, Löwe B. Screening psychischer Störungen mit dem “Gesundheitsfragebogen für Patienten (PHQ-D)“. Diagnostica 2004; 50(4):171–81.
  43. 43. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown G. Beck Depression Inventory–II (BDI-II). APA PsycTests; 1996.
  44. 44. Kühner C, Bürger C, Keller F, Hautzinger M. Reliabilität und Validität des revidierten Beck-Depressionsinventars (BDI-II). Befunde aus deutschsprachigen Stichproben. Nervenarzt 2007; 78(6):651–6.
  45. 45. Kemper CJ, Ziegler M, Taylor S. ASI-3—Angstsensitivitätsindex-3; 2011.
  46. 46. Taylor S, Zvolensky MJ, Cox BJ, Deacon B, Heimberg RG, Ledley DR et al. Robust dimensions of anxiety sensitivity: development and initial validation of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3. Psychol Assess 2007; 19(2):176–88. pmid:17563199
  47. 47. Spielberger CD, Gorusch RL, Lushene RE. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologist Press; 1970.
  48. 48. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows. IBM Corp.; 2020.
  49. 49. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide. Eighth Edition. Muthén & Muthén; 1998–2017.
  50. 50. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria; 2013. Available from: URL: http://www.R-project.org/.
  51. 51. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. J. Stat. Soft. 2015; 67(1).
  52. 52. Diedenhofen B, Musch J. cocor: a comprehensive solution for the statistical comparison of correlations. PLoS One 2015; 10(3):e0121945. pmid:25835001
  53. 53. Wickham H, Averick M, Bryan J, Chang W, McGowan L, François R et al. Welcome to the Tidyverse. JOSS 2019; 4(43):1686.
  54. 54. Revelle W. Psych: procedures for psychological, psychometric, and personality research 2020.
  55. 55. Rosseel Y. lavaan: An R Package for Structural Equation Modeling. J. Stat. Soft. 2012; 48(2).
  56. 56. Gonzalez I, Déjean S, Martin P, Baccini A. CCA: An R Package to Extend Canonical Correlation Analysis. J. Stat. Soft. 2008; 23(12).
  57. 57. Muthén BO. A general structural equation model with dichotomous, ordered categorical, and continuous latent variable indicators. Psychometrika 1984; 49:115–32.
  58. 58. Hooper D, Coughlan J, Mullen MR. Structural Equation Modelling: Guidelines for Determining Model Fit. The Electronic Journal of Business Research Methods 2008; 6:53–60.
  59. 59. Schermelleh-Engel K, Moosbrugger H, Müller H. Evaluating the Fit of Structural Equation Models: Tests of Significance and Descriptive Goodness-of-Fit Measures. Methods of Psychological Research 2003; 8(2):23–74.
  60. 60. Comrey AL, Lee HB. A First Course in Factor Analysis. Psychology Press; 2013.
  61. 61. Murphy J, Brewer R, Catmur C, Bird G. Interoception and psychopathology: A developmental neuroscience perspective. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2017; 23:45–56. pmid:28081519
  62. 62. Grabauskaitė A, Baranauskas M, Griškova-Bulanova I. Interoception and gender: What aspects should we pay attention to? Conscious Cogn 2017; 48:129–37. pmid:27866005
  63. 63. Prentice F, Murphy J. Sex differences in interoceptive accuracy: A meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2022; 132:497–518. pmid:34838927
  64. 64. Kolacz J, Chen X, Nix EJ, Roath OK, Holmes LG, Tokash C et al. Association of Self-Reported Autonomic Symptoms with Sensor-Based Physiological Measures. Psychosom Med 2023. pmid:37678358
  65. 65. Wendt J, Hamm AO, Pané-Farré CA, Thayer JF, Gerlach A, Gloster AT et al. Pretreatment Cardiac Vagal Tone Predicts Dropout from and Residual Symptoms after Exposure Therapy in Patients with Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia. Psychother Psychosom 2018; 87(3):187–9. pmid:29533952
  66. 66. Laborde S, Ackermann S, Borges U, D’Agostini M, Giraudier M, Iskra M et al. Leveraging Vagally Mediated Heart Rate Variability as an Actionable, Noninvasive Biomarker for Self-Regulation: Assessment, Intervention, and Evaluation. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2023; 10(2):212–20.
  67. 67. Barrett LF, Quigley KS, Hamilton P. An active inference theory of allostasis and interoception in depression. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2016; 371(1708). pmid:28080969
  68. 68. Ventura-Bort C, Weymar M. Transcutaneous auricular vagus nerve stimulation modulates the processing of interoceptive prediction error signals and their role in allostatic regulation. Hum Brain Mapp. 2024;45(3):e26613. pmid:38379451
  69. 69. Brand S, Petzke TM, Witthöft M. The Differential Relationship Between Self-Reported Interoceptive Accuracy and Attention With Psychopathology. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie 2022; 51(3–4):165–75.